Interrupted incubation: How dabbling ducks respond when flushed from the nest
Abstract Nesting birds must provide a thermal environment sufficient for egg development while also meeting self‐maintenance needs. Many birds, particularly those with uniparental incubation, achieve this balance through periodic incubation recesses, during which foraging and other self‐maintenance...
Main Authors: | , , , , , , , , |
---|---|
Format: | Article |
Language: | English |
Published: |
Wiley
2021-03-01
|
Series: | Ecology and Evolution |
Subjects: | |
Online Access: | https://doi.org/10.1002/ece3.7245 |
_version_ | 1818576315883716608 |
---|---|
author | Rebecca Croston C. Alex Hartman Mark P. Herzog Sarah H. Peterson Jeffrey D. Kohl Cory T. Overton Cliff L. Feldheim Michael L. Casazza Joshua T. Ackerman |
author_facet | Rebecca Croston C. Alex Hartman Mark P. Herzog Sarah H. Peterson Jeffrey D. Kohl Cory T. Overton Cliff L. Feldheim Michael L. Casazza Joshua T. Ackerman |
author_sort | Rebecca Croston |
collection | DOAJ |
description | Abstract Nesting birds must provide a thermal environment sufficient for egg development while also meeting self‐maintenance needs. Many birds, particularly those with uniparental incubation, achieve this balance through periodic incubation recesses, during which foraging and other self‐maintenance activities can occur. However, incubating birds may experience disturbances such as predator or human activity which interrupt natural incubation patterns by compelling them to leave the nest. We characterized incubating mallard Anas platyrhynchos and gadwall Mareca strepera hens’ responses when flushed by predators and investigators in Suisun Marsh, California, USA. Diurnal incubation recesses initiated by investigators approaching nests were 63% longer than natural diurnal incubation recesses initiated by the hen (geometric mean: 226.77 min versus 142.04 min). Nocturnal incubation recesses, many of which were likely the result of predators flushing hens, were of similar duration regardless of whether the nest was partially depredated during the event (115.33 [101.01;131.68] minutes) or not (119.62 [111.96;127.82] minutes), yet were 16% shorter than natural diurnal incubation recesses. Hens moved further from the nest during natural diurnal recesses or investigator‐initiated recesses than during nocturnal recesses, and the proportion of hen locations recorded in wetland versus upland habitat during recesses varied with recess type (model‐predicted means: natural diurnal recess 0.77; investigator‐initiated recess 0.82; nocturnal recess 0.31). Hens were more likely to take a natural recess following an investigator‐initiated recess earlier that same day than following a natural recess earlier that same day, and natural recesses that followed an investigator‐initiated recess were longer than natural recesses that followed an earlier natural recess, suggesting that hens may not fulfill all of their physiological needs during investigator‐initiated recesses. We found no evidence that the duration of investigator‐initiated recesses was influenced by repeated visits to the nest, whether by predators or by investigators, and trapping and handling the hen did not affect investigator‐initiated recess duration unless the hen was also fitted with a backpack‐harness style GPS–GSM transmitter at the time of capture. Hens that were captured and fitted with GPS–GSM transmitters took recesses that were 26% longer than recesses during which a hen was captured but a GPS–GSM transmitter was not attached. Incubation interruptions had measurable but limited and specific effects on hen behavior. |
first_indexed | 2024-12-16T06:12:04Z |
format | Article |
id | doaj.art-2500067365774946a1b827aea8ac807c |
institution | Directory Open Access Journal |
issn | 2045-7758 |
language | English |
last_indexed | 2024-12-16T06:12:04Z |
publishDate | 2021-03-01 |
publisher | Wiley |
record_format | Article |
series | Ecology and Evolution |
spelling | doaj.art-2500067365774946a1b827aea8ac807c2022-12-21T22:41:21ZengWileyEcology and Evolution2045-77582021-03-011162862287210.1002/ece3.7245Interrupted incubation: How dabbling ducks respond when flushed from the nestRebecca Croston0C. Alex Hartman1Mark P. Herzog2Sarah H. Peterson3Jeffrey D. Kohl4Cory T. Overton5Cliff L. Feldheim6Michael L. Casazza7Joshua T. Ackerman8U.S. Geological Survey Western Ecological Research Center Dixon CA USAU.S. Geological Survey Western Ecological Research Center Dixon CA USAU.S. Geological Survey Western Ecological Research Center Dixon CA USAU.S. Geological Survey Western Ecological Research Center Dixon CA USAU.S. Geological Survey Western Ecological Research Center Dixon CA USAU.S. Geological Survey Western Ecological Research Center Dixon CA USACalifornia Department of Water Resources Suisun Marsh Program West Sacramento CA USAU.S. Geological Survey Western Ecological Research Center Dixon CA USAU.S. Geological Survey Western Ecological Research Center Dixon CA USAAbstract Nesting birds must provide a thermal environment sufficient for egg development while also meeting self‐maintenance needs. Many birds, particularly those with uniparental incubation, achieve this balance through periodic incubation recesses, during which foraging and other self‐maintenance activities can occur. However, incubating birds may experience disturbances such as predator or human activity which interrupt natural incubation patterns by compelling them to leave the nest. We characterized incubating mallard Anas platyrhynchos and gadwall Mareca strepera hens’ responses when flushed by predators and investigators in Suisun Marsh, California, USA. Diurnal incubation recesses initiated by investigators approaching nests were 63% longer than natural diurnal incubation recesses initiated by the hen (geometric mean: 226.77 min versus 142.04 min). Nocturnal incubation recesses, many of which were likely the result of predators flushing hens, were of similar duration regardless of whether the nest was partially depredated during the event (115.33 [101.01;131.68] minutes) or not (119.62 [111.96;127.82] minutes), yet were 16% shorter than natural diurnal incubation recesses. Hens moved further from the nest during natural diurnal recesses or investigator‐initiated recesses than during nocturnal recesses, and the proportion of hen locations recorded in wetland versus upland habitat during recesses varied with recess type (model‐predicted means: natural diurnal recess 0.77; investigator‐initiated recess 0.82; nocturnal recess 0.31). Hens were more likely to take a natural recess following an investigator‐initiated recess earlier that same day than following a natural recess earlier that same day, and natural recesses that followed an investigator‐initiated recess were longer than natural recesses that followed an earlier natural recess, suggesting that hens may not fulfill all of their physiological needs during investigator‐initiated recesses. We found no evidence that the duration of investigator‐initiated recesses was influenced by repeated visits to the nest, whether by predators or by investigators, and trapping and handling the hen did not affect investigator‐initiated recess duration unless the hen was also fitted with a backpack‐harness style GPS–GSM transmitter at the time of capture. Hens that were captured and fitted with GPS–GSM transmitters took recesses that were 26% longer than recesses during which a hen was captured but a GPS–GSM transmitter was not attached. Incubation interruptions had measurable but limited and specific effects on hen behavior.https://doi.org/10.1002/ece3.7245dabbling duckdisturbancegadwallGPS–GSM transmitteriButtonincubation recess |
spellingShingle | Rebecca Croston C. Alex Hartman Mark P. Herzog Sarah H. Peterson Jeffrey D. Kohl Cory T. Overton Cliff L. Feldheim Michael L. Casazza Joshua T. Ackerman Interrupted incubation: How dabbling ducks respond when flushed from the nest Ecology and Evolution dabbling duck disturbance gadwall GPS–GSM transmitter iButton incubation recess |
title | Interrupted incubation: How dabbling ducks respond when flushed from the nest |
title_full | Interrupted incubation: How dabbling ducks respond when flushed from the nest |
title_fullStr | Interrupted incubation: How dabbling ducks respond when flushed from the nest |
title_full_unstemmed | Interrupted incubation: How dabbling ducks respond when flushed from the nest |
title_short | Interrupted incubation: How dabbling ducks respond when flushed from the nest |
title_sort | interrupted incubation how dabbling ducks respond when flushed from the nest |
topic | dabbling duck disturbance gadwall GPS–GSM transmitter iButton incubation recess |
url | https://doi.org/10.1002/ece3.7245 |
work_keys_str_mv | AT rebeccacroston interruptedincubationhowdabblingducksrespondwhenflushedfromthenest AT calexhartman interruptedincubationhowdabblingducksrespondwhenflushedfromthenest AT markpherzog interruptedincubationhowdabblingducksrespondwhenflushedfromthenest AT sarahhpeterson interruptedincubationhowdabblingducksrespondwhenflushedfromthenest AT jeffreydkohl interruptedincubationhowdabblingducksrespondwhenflushedfromthenest AT corytoverton interruptedincubationhowdabblingducksrespondwhenflushedfromthenest AT clifflfeldheim interruptedincubationhowdabblingducksrespondwhenflushedfromthenest AT michaellcasazza interruptedincubationhowdabblingducksrespondwhenflushedfromthenest AT joshuatackerman interruptedincubationhowdabblingducksrespondwhenflushedfromthenest |