Omit needless words: Sentence length perception

Short sentences improve readability. Short sentences also promote social justice through accessibility and inclusiveness. Despite this, much remains unknown about sentence length perception—an important factor in producing readable writing. Accordingly, we conducted a psychophysical study using proc...

Full description

Bibliographic Details
Main Authors: Nestor Matthews, Folly Folivi
Format: Article
Language:English
Published: Public Library of Science (PLoS) 2023-01-01
Series:PLoS ONE
Online Access:https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC9955962/?tool=EBI
_version_ 1797894314043375616
author Nestor Matthews
Folly Folivi
author_facet Nestor Matthews
Folly Folivi
author_sort Nestor Matthews
collection DOAJ
description Short sentences improve readability. Short sentences also promote social justice through accessibility and inclusiveness. Despite this, much remains unknown about sentence length perception—an important factor in producing readable writing. Accordingly, we conducted a psychophysical study using procedures from Signal Detection Theory to examine sentence length perception in naive adults. Participants viewed real-world full-page text samples and judged whether a bolded target sentence contained more or fewer than 17 words. The experiment yielded four findings. First, naïve adults perceived sentence length in real-world text samples quickly (median = 300–400 ms) and precisely (median = ~90% correct). Second, flipping real-world text samples upside-down generated no reaction-time cost and nearly no loss in the precision of sentence length perception. This differs from the large inversion effects that characterize other highly practiced, real-world perceptual tasks involving canonically oriented stimuli, most notably face perception and reading. Third, participants significantly underestimated the length of mirror-reversed sentences—but not upside-down, nor standard sentences. This finding parallels participants’ familiarity with commonly occurring left-justified right-ragged text, and suggests a novel demonstration of left-lateralized anchoring in scene syntax. Fourth, error patterns demonstrated that participants achieved their high speed, high precision sentence-length judgments by heuristically counting text lines, not by explicitly counting words. This suggests practical advice for writing instructors to offer students. When copy editing, students can quickly and precisely identify their long sentences via a line-counting heuristic, e.g., “a 17-word sentence spans about 1.5 text lines”. Students can subsequently improve a long sentence’s readability and inclusiveness by omitting needless words.
first_indexed 2024-04-10T07:06:57Z
format Article
id doaj.art-299cf354ab05483391723670a78c3690
institution Directory Open Access Journal
issn 1932-6203
language English
last_indexed 2024-04-10T07:06:57Z
publishDate 2023-01-01
publisher Public Library of Science (PLoS)
record_format Article
series PLoS ONE
spelling doaj.art-299cf354ab05483391723670a78c36902023-02-27T05:31:23ZengPublic Library of Science (PLoS)PLoS ONE1932-62032023-01-01182Omit needless words: Sentence length perceptionNestor MatthewsFolly FoliviShort sentences improve readability. Short sentences also promote social justice through accessibility and inclusiveness. Despite this, much remains unknown about sentence length perception—an important factor in producing readable writing. Accordingly, we conducted a psychophysical study using procedures from Signal Detection Theory to examine sentence length perception in naive adults. Participants viewed real-world full-page text samples and judged whether a bolded target sentence contained more or fewer than 17 words. The experiment yielded four findings. First, naïve adults perceived sentence length in real-world text samples quickly (median = 300–400 ms) and precisely (median = ~90% correct). Second, flipping real-world text samples upside-down generated no reaction-time cost and nearly no loss in the precision of sentence length perception. This differs from the large inversion effects that characterize other highly practiced, real-world perceptual tasks involving canonically oriented stimuli, most notably face perception and reading. Third, participants significantly underestimated the length of mirror-reversed sentences—but not upside-down, nor standard sentences. This finding parallels participants’ familiarity with commonly occurring left-justified right-ragged text, and suggests a novel demonstration of left-lateralized anchoring in scene syntax. Fourth, error patterns demonstrated that participants achieved their high speed, high precision sentence-length judgments by heuristically counting text lines, not by explicitly counting words. This suggests practical advice for writing instructors to offer students. When copy editing, students can quickly and precisely identify their long sentences via a line-counting heuristic, e.g., “a 17-word sentence spans about 1.5 text lines”. Students can subsequently improve a long sentence’s readability and inclusiveness by omitting needless words.https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC9955962/?tool=EBI
spellingShingle Nestor Matthews
Folly Folivi
Omit needless words: Sentence length perception
PLoS ONE
title Omit needless words: Sentence length perception
title_full Omit needless words: Sentence length perception
title_fullStr Omit needless words: Sentence length perception
title_full_unstemmed Omit needless words: Sentence length perception
title_short Omit needless words: Sentence length perception
title_sort omit needless words sentence length perception
url https://www.ncbi.nlm.nih.gov/pmc/articles/PMC9955962/?tool=EBI
work_keys_str_mv AT nestormatthews omitneedlesswordssentencelengthperception
AT follyfolivi omitneedlesswordssentencelengthperception