Structure and Dynamics of Humpback Whales Competitive Groups in Ecuador
We assessed the social structure and behavior of humpback whale (Megaptera novaeangliae) competitive groups off Ecuador between July and August 2010. During this time we followed 185 whales in 22 competitive groups for 41.45 hr. The average group size was 8.4 animals (SD = 2.85). The average sightin...
Main Authors: | , |
---|---|
Format: | Article |
Language: | English |
Published: |
Animal Behavior and Cognition
2015-02-01
|
Series: | Animal Behavior and Cognition |
Subjects: | |
Online Access: | http://www.animalbehaviorandcognition.org/uploads/journals/5/05.Felix_Novillo_FINAL.pdf |
_version_ | 1818501924598579200 |
---|---|
author | Fernando Félix Johana Novillo |
author_facet | Fernando Félix Johana Novillo |
author_sort | Fernando Félix |
collection | DOAJ |
description | We assessed the social structure and behavior of humpback whale (Megaptera novaeangliae) competitive groups off Ecuador between July and August 2010. During this time we followed 185 whales in 22 competitive groups for 41.45 hr. The average group size was 8.4 animals (SD = 2.85). The average sighting time was 113.05 min/group (SD = 47.1). We used photographs of dorsal fins and video to record interactions and estimate an association index (AI) between each pair of whales within the groups. Sightings were divided into periods, which were defined by changes in group membership. On average, group composition changed every 30.2 min, which confirms that the structure of competitive groups is highly dynamic. Interactions between escorts characterized by low level of aggression. At least 60% of escorts joined or left together the group in small subunits between two and five animals, suggesting some type of cooperative association. Although singletons, as well as pairs or trios were able to join competitive groups at any moment, escorts that joined together were able to stay longer with the group and displace dominant escorts. Genetic analysis showed that in three occasions more than one female was present within a competitive group, suggesting either males are herding females or large competitive groups are formed by subunits. Males and females performed similar surface displays. We propose that competition and cooperation are interrelated in humpback whales’ competitive groups and that male cooperation would be an adaptive strategy either to displace dominant escorts or to fend off challengers. |
first_indexed | 2024-12-10T21:02:58Z |
format | Article |
id | doaj.art-3345f0f46b41460dbaad3a4dc55a2c40 |
institution | Directory Open Access Journal |
issn | 2372-5052 2372-4323 |
language | English |
last_indexed | 2024-12-10T21:02:58Z |
publishDate | 2015-02-01 |
publisher | Animal Behavior and Cognition |
record_format | Article |
series | Animal Behavior and Cognition |
spelling | doaj.art-3345f0f46b41460dbaad3a4dc55a2c402022-12-22T01:33:44ZengAnimal Behavior and CognitionAnimal Behavior and Cognition2372-50522372-43232015-02-0121567010.12966/abc.02.05.2015Structure and Dynamics of Humpback Whales Competitive Groups in EcuadorFernando FélixJohana NovilloWe assessed the social structure and behavior of humpback whale (Megaptera novaeangliae) competitive groups off Ecuador between July and August 2010. During this time we followed 185 whales in 22 competitive groups for 41.45 hr. The average group size was 8.4 animals (SD = 2.85). The average sighting time was 113.05 min/group (SD = 47.1). We used photographs of dorsal fins and video to record interactions and estimate an association index (AI) between each pair of whales within the groups. Sightings were divided into periods, which were defined by changes in group membership. On average, group composition changed every 30.2 min, which confirms that the structure of competitive groups is highly dynamic. Interactions between escorts characterized by low level of aggression. At least 60% of escorts joined or left together the group in small subunits between two and five animals, suggesting some type of cooperative association. Although singletons, as well as pairs or trios were able to join competitive groups at any moment, escorts that joined together were able to stay longer with the group and displace dominant escorts. Genetic analysis showed that in three occasions more than one female was present within a competitive group, suggesting either males are herding females or large competitive groups are formed by subunits. Males and females performed similar surface displays. We propose that competition and cooperation are interrelated in humpback whales’ competitive groups and that male cooperation would be an adaptive strategy either to displace dominant escorts or to fend off challengers.http://www.animalbehaviorandcognition.org/uploads/journals/5/05.Felix_Novillo_FINAL.pdfBehaviorCompetitive groupsCooperationHumpback whalesMegaptera novaeangliae |
spellingShingle | Fernando Félix Johana Novillo Structure and Dynamics of Humpback Whales Competitive Groups in Ecuador Animal Behavior and Cognition Behavior Competitive groups Cooperation Humpback whales Megaptera novaeangliae |
title | Structure and Dynamics of Humpback Whales Competitive Groups in Ecuador |
title_full | Structure and Dynamics of Humpback Whales Competitive Groups in Ecuador |
title_fullStr | Structure and Dynamics of Humpback Whales Competitive Groups in Ecuador |
title_full_unstemmed | Structure and Dynamics of Humpback Whales Competitive Groups in Ecuador |
title_short | Structure and Dynamics of Humpback Whales Competitive Groups in Ecuador |
title_sort | structure and dynamics of humpback whales competitive groups in ecuador |
topic | Behavior Competitive groups Cooperation Humpback whales Megaptera novaeangliae |
url | http://www.animalbehaviorandcognition.org/uploads/journals/5/05.Felix_Novillo_FINAL.pdf |
work_keys_str_mv | AT fernandofelix structureanddynamicsofhumpbackwhalescompetitivegroupsinecuador AT johananovillo structureanddynamicsofhumpbackwhalescompetitivegroupsinecuador |