A direct replication and extension of Popp and Serra (2016, experiment 1): better free recall and worse cued recall of animal names than object names, accounting for semantic similarity
IntroductionFree recall tends to be better for names of animate concepts such as animals than for names of inanimate objects. In Popp and Serra’s 2016 article, the authors replicated this “animacy effect” in free recall but when participants studied words in pairs (animate-animate pairs intermixed w...
Main Authors: | , , , , , |
---|---|
Format: | Article |
Language: | English |
Published: |
Frontiers Media S.A.
2023-05-01
|
Series: | Frontiers in Psychology |
Subjects: | |
Online Access: | https://www.frontiersin.org/articles/10.3389/fpsyg.2023.1146200/full |
_version_ | 1827944676872683520 |
---|---|
author | Eric Y. Mah Kelly E. L. Grannon Alison Campbell Nicholas Tamburri Randall K. Jamieson D. Stephen Lindsay |
author_facet | Eric Y. Mah Kelly E. L. Grannon Alison Campbell Nicholas Tamburri Randall K. Jamieson D. Stephen Lindsay |
author_sort | Eric Y. Mah |
collection | DOAJ |
description | IntroductionFree recall tends to be better for names of animate concepts such as animals than for names of inanimate objects. In Popp and Serra’s 2016 article, the authors replicated this “animacy effect” in free recall but when participants studied words in pairs (animate-animate pairs intermixed with inanimate-inanimate pairs) and were tested with cued recall, performance was better for inanimate-inanimate pairs than for animate-animate pairs (“reverse animacy”). We tested the replicability of this surprising effect and one possible explanation for the effect (semantic similarity).MethodsOur Experiment 1 was a preregistered direct replication (N = 101) of Popp and Serra’s Experiment 1 (mixed-lists condition). In a second preregistered experiment conducted in four different samples (undergraduate N = 153, undergraduate N = 143, online Prolific N = 101, online Prolific/English-as-a-first-language N = 150), we manipulated the within-category semantic similarity of animal and object wordlists.ResultsAIn Experiment 1, just as in Popp and Serra, we observed an animacy effect for free recall and a reverse animacy effect for cued recall. Unlike Popp and Serra, we found that controlling for interference effects rendered the reverse animacy effect non-significant. We took this as evidence that characteristics of the stimulus sets (e.g., category structure, within-category similarity) may play a role in animacy and reverse animacy effects. In Experiment 2, in three out of our four samples, we observed reverse animacy effects when within-category similarity was higher for animals and when within-category similarity was equated for animals and objects.DiscussionOur results suggest that the reverse animacy effect observed in Popp and Serra’s 2016 article is a robust and replicable effect, but that semantic similarity alone cannot explain the effect. |
first_indexed | 2024-03-13T10:33:54Z |
format | Article |
id | doaj.art-430a305fb2b44c059c1eaf05373622f8 |
institution | Directory Open Access Journal |
issn | 1664-1078 |
language | English |
last_indexed | 2024-03-13T10:33:54Z |
publishDate | 2023-05-01 |
publisher | Frontiers Media S.A. |
record_format | Article |
series | Frontiers in Psychology |
spelling | doaj.art-430a305fb2b44c059c1eaf05373622f82023-05-18T08:03:22ZengFrontiers Media S.A.Frontiers in Psychology1664-10782023-05-011410.3389/fpsyg.2023.11462001146200A direct replication and extension of Popp and Serra (2016, experiment 1): better free recall and worse cued recall of animal names than object names, accounting for semantic similarityEric Y. Mah0Kelly E. L. Grannon1Alison Campbell2Nicholas Tamburri3Randall K. Jamieson4D. Stephen Lindsay5Department of Psychology, University of Victoria, Victoria, BC, CanadaDepartment of Psychology, University of Victoria, Victoria, BC, CanadaDepartment of Psychology, University of Victoria, Victoria, BC, CanadaDepartment of Psychology, University of Victoria, Victoria, BC, CanadaDepartment of Psychology, University of Manitoba, Winnipeg, MB, CanadaDepartment of Psychology, University of Victoria, Victoria, BC, CanadaIntroductionFree recall tends to be better for names of animate concepts such as animals than for names of inanimate objects. In Popp and Serra’s 2016 article, the authors replicated this “animacy effect” in free recall but when participants studied words in pairs (animate-animate pairs intermixed with inanimate-inanimate pairs) and were tested with cued recall, performance was better for inanimate-inanimate pairs than for animate-animate pairs (“reverse animacy”). We tested the replicability of this surprising effect and one possible explanation for the effect (semantic similarity).MethodsOur Experiment 1 was a preregistered direct replication (N = 101) of Popp and Serra’s Experiment 1 (mixed-lists condition). In a second preregistered experiment conducted in four different samples (undergraduate N = 153, undergraduate N = 143, online Prolific N = 101, online Prolific/English-as-a-first-language N = 150), we manipulated the within-category semantic similarity of animal and object wordlists.ResultsAIn Experiment 1, just as in Popp and Serra, we observed an animacy effect for free recall and a reverse animacy effect for cued recall. Unlike Popp and Serra, we found that controlling for interference effects rendered the reverse animacy effect non-significant. We took this as evidence that characteristics of the stimulus sets (e.g., category structure, within-category similarity) may play a role in animacy and reverse animacy effects. In Experiment 2, in three out of our four samples, we observed reverse animacy effects when within-category similarity was higher for animals and when within-category similarity was equated for animals and objects.DiscussionOur results suggest that the reverse animacy effect observed in Popp and Serra’s 2016 article is a robust and replicable effect, but that semantic similarity alone cannot explain the effect.https://www.frontiersin.org/articles/10.3389/fpsyg.2023.1146200/fulladaptive memoryanimacycued recallfree recalldirect replication |
spellingShingle | Eric Y. Mah Kelly E. L. Grannon Alison Campbell Nicholas Tamburri Randall K. Jamieson D. Stephen Lindsay A direct replication and extension of Popp and Serra (2016, experiment 1): better free recall and worse cued recall of animal names than object names, accounting for semantic similarity Frontiers in Psychology adaptive memory animacy cued recall free recall direct replication |
title | A direct replication and extension of Popp and Serra (2016, experiment 1): better free recall and worse cued recall of animal names than object names, accounting for semantic similarity |
title_full | A direct replication and extension of Popp and Serra (2016, experiment 1): better free recall and worse cued recall of animal names than object names, accounting for semantic similarity |
title_fullStr | A direct replication and extension of Popp and Serra (2016, experiment 1): better free recall and worse cued recall of animal names than object names, accounting for semantic similarity |
title_full_unstemmed | A direct replication and extension of Popp and Serra (2016, experiment 1): better free recall and worse cued recall of animal names than object names, accounting for semantic similarity |
title_short | A direct replication and extension of Popp and Serra (2016, experiment 1): better free recall and worse cued recall of animal names than object names, accounting for semantic similarity |
title_sort | direct replication and extension of popp and serra 2016 experiment 1 better free recall and worse cued recall of animal names than object names accounting for semantic similarity |
topic | adaptive memory animacy cued recall free recall direct replication |
url | https://www.frontiersin.org/articles/10.3389/fpsyg.2023.1146200/full |
work_keys_str_mv | AT ericymah adirectreplicationandextensionofpoppandserra2016experiment1betterfreerecallandworsecuedrecallofanimalnamesthanobjectnamesaccountingforsemanticsimilarity AT kellyelgrannon adirectreplicationandextensionofpoppandserra2016experiment1betterfreerecallandworsecuedrecallofanimalnamesthanobjectnamesaccountingforsemanticsimilarity AT alisoncampbell adirectreplicationandextensionofpoppandserra2016experiment1betterfreerecallandworsecuedrecallofanimalnamesthanobjectnamesaccountingforsemanticsimilarity AT nicholastamburri adirectreplicationandextensionofpoppandserra2016experiment1betterfreerecallandworsecuedrecallofanimalnamesthanobjectnamesaccountingforsemanticsimilarity AT randallkjamieson adirectreplicationandextensionofpoppandserra2016experiment1betterfreerecallandworsecuedrecallofanimalnamesthanobjectnamesaccountingforsemanticsimilarity AT dstephenlindsay adirectreplicationandextensionofpoppandserra2016experiment1betterfreerecallandworsecuedrecallofanimalnamesthanobjectnamesaccountingforsemanticsimilarity AT ericymah directreplicationandextensionofpoppandserra2016experiment1betterfreerecallandworsecuedrecallofanimalnamesthanobjectnamesaccountingforsemanticsimilarity AT kellyelgrannon directreplicationandextensionofpoppandserra2016experiment1betterfreerecallandworsecuedrecallofanimalnamesthanobjectnamesaccountingforsemanticsimilarity AT alisoncampbell directreplicationandextensionofpoppandserra2016experiment1betterfreerecallandworsecuedrecallofanimalnamesthanobjectnamesaccountingforsemanticsimilarity AT nicholastamburri directreplicationandextensionofpoppandserra2016experiment1betterfreerecallandworsecuedrecallofanimalnamesthanobjectnamesaccountingforsemanticsimilarity AT randallkjamieson directreplicationandextensionofpoppandserra2016experiment1betterfreerecallandworsecuedrecallofanimalnamesthanobjectnamesaccountingforsemanticsimilarity AT dstephenlindsay directreplicationandextensionofpoppandserra2016experiment1betterfreerecallandworsecuedrecallofanimalnamesthanobjectnamesaccountingforsemanticsimilarity |