Does auditory attentional bias determine craving for methamphetamine? A pilot study using a word recognition dichotic listening task

Background: Obtaining reliable data under explicit evaluations is one of the most complicated challenges in assessing drug users' status. Respondents are likely to give answers that are to their advantage or deliberately deceitful. Regarding drug use, intense and inevitable drug craving is know...

Full description

Bibliographic Details
Main Authors: Maryam Kazemitabar, Mohammad Taghi Kheirkhah, Mehran Mokarrami, Danilo Garcia
Format: Article
Language:English
Published: Elsevier 2022-11-01
Series:Heliyon
Subjects:
Online Access:http://www.sciencedirect.com/science/article/pii/S2405844022025993
_version_ 1811213612323176448
author Maryam Kazemitabar
Mohammad Taghi Kheirkhah
Mehran Mokarrami
Danilo Garcia
author_facet Maryam Kazemitabar
Mohammad Taghi Kheirkhah
Mehran Mokarrami
Danilo Garcia
author_sort Maryam Kazemitabar
collection DOAJ
description Background: Obtaining reliable data under explicit evaluations is one of the most complicated challenges in assessing drug users' status. Respondents are likely to give answers that are to their advantage or deliberately deceitful. Regarding drug use, intense and inevitable drug craving is known as one of the main causes of relapse and treatment failure. As a matter of fact, drug craving is directly correlated to attentional bias toward drug-related stimuli, while drug-related stimuli capture drug users' attention as a result of craving. Most methods for studying selective attention and attentional bias have been developed for visual modality. However, stimuli that capture drug users’ attention are not always visual, they could be auditory. Aims: We examined if a modified word recognition dichotic listening task discriminated between methamphetamine users and non-users. Moreover, we investigated further the reliability and validity of this new paradigm. Methods: A total of 30 adult males participated in the study (15 methamphetamine users and 15 non-users). The word recognition dichotic listening task included two stimuli narratives/sequences (one neutral and one methamphetamine-related) that were presented simultaneously via headphones, one stimuli sequence to each ear. The participants were instructed to only pay attention to the neutral stimuli and to ignore the drug-related stimuli. Afterward, participants were asked to indicate in a list which words they recognized from the listening task and responded to the Desire for Drug Questionnaire, which was modified to address methamphetamine craving. In addition, a month after the experiment, we assessed therapy adherence among participants who were methamphetamine users. Results: Methamphetamine users had a significantly lower performance in the word recognition dichotic task compared to non-users (t = 4.30, p < .001; Cohen’s d = 6.13). Importantly, the average performance on the task was significantly higher among methamphetamine users who continued their treatment one month later compared to those who quitted (t = −2.56, p < .05; Hedges' g = 1.28). Moreover, the intraclass correlation coefficient with 95% interval confidence for the word recognition dichotic listening task scores was excellent (ICC = 0.90) and the scores were significantly correlated with self-reported methamphetamine craving (r = −.47, p < .001). Conclusions: The modified word recognition dichotic listening task successfully discriminated between individuals who craved methamphetamine from those who did not. This new paradigm demonstrated high reliability and validity in the present pilot study. Due to the importance of preventing unreliable responses when assessing drug cravings, the current method can be, after further validation, utilized in both research and clinical practices.
first_indexed 2024-04-12T05:49:34Z
format Article
id doaj.art-4fac6c5ab64c45209f392bfbe26c5e9a
institution Directory Open Access Journal
issn 2405-8440
language English
last_indexed 2024-04-12T05:49:34Z
publishDate 2022-11-01
publisher Elsevier
record_format Article
series Heliyon
spelling doaj.art-4fac6c5ab64c45209f392bfbe26c5e9a2022-12-22T03:45:20ZengElsevierHeliyon2405-84402022-11-01811e11311Does auditory attentional bias determine craving for methamphetamine? A pilot study using a word recognition dichotic listening taskMaryam Kazemitabar0Mohammad Taghi Kheirkhah1Mehran Mokarrami2Danilo Garcia3Yale University School of Medicine, New Haven, CT, United States; Promotion of Health and Innovation (PHI) Lab, International Network for Well-Being, United StatesInstitute for Cognitive and Brain Sciences, Shahid Beheshti University, Tehran, Iran; Corresponding author:Department of Psychology, University of Tehran, Tehran, Iran; Corresponding author:Department of Behavioral Sciences and Learning, Linköping University, Linköping, Sweden; Centre for Ethics, Law and Mental Health (CELAM), University of Gothenburg, Gothenburg, Sweden; Promotion of Health and Innovation (PHI) Lab, International Network for Well-Being, Sweden; Department of Psychology, Lund University, Lund, Sweden; Department of Psychology, University of Gothenburg, Gothenburg, Sweden; Corresponding author.Background: Obtaining reliable data under explicit evaluations is one of the most complicated challenges in assessing drug users' status. Respondents are likely to give answers that are to their advantage or deliberately deceitful. Regarding drug use, intense and inevitable drug craving is known as one of the main causes of relapse and treatment failure. As a matter of fact, drug craving is directly correlated to attentional bias toward drug-related stimuli, while drug-related stimuli capture drug users' attention as a result of craving. Most methods for studying selective attention and attentional bias have been developed for visual modality. However, stimuli that capture drug users’ attention are not always visual, they could be auditory. Aims: We examined if a modified word recognition dichotic listening task discriminated between methamphetamine users and non-users. Moreover, we investigated further the reliability and validity of this new paradigm. Methods: A total of 30 adult males participated in the study (15 methamphetamine users and 15 non-users). The word recognition dichotic listening task included two stimuli narratives/sequences (one neutral and one methamphetamine-related) that were presented simultaneously via headphones, one stimuli sequence to each ear. The participants were instructed to only pay attention to the neutral stimuli and to ignore the drug-related stimuli. Afterward, participants were asked to indicate in a list which words they recognized from the listening task and responded to the Desire for Drug Questionnaire, which was modified to address methamphetamine craving. In addition, a month after the experiment, we assessed therapy adherence among participants who were methamphetamine users. Results: Methamphetamine users had a significantly lower performance in the word recognition dichotic task compared to non-users (t = 4.30, p < .001; Cohen’s d = 6.13). Importantly, the average performance on the task was significantly higher among methamphetamine users who continued their treatment one month later compared to those who quitted (t = −2.56, p < .05; Hedges' g = 1.28). Moreover, the intraclass correlation coefficient with 95% interval confidence for the word recognition dichotic listening task scores was excellent (ICC = 0.90) and the scores were significantly correlated with self-reported methamphetamine craving (r = −.47, p < .001). Conclusions: The modified word recognition dichotic listening task successfully discriminated between individuals who craved methamphetamine from those who did not. This new paradigm demonstrated high reliability and validity in the present pilot study. Due to the importance of preventing unreliable responses when assessing drug cravings, the current method can be, after further validation, utilized in both research and clinical practices.http://www.sciencedirect.com/science/article/pii/S2405844022025993Methamphetamine cravingImplicit assessmentAttentional biasDichotic listening paradigm
spellingShingle Maryam Kazemitabar
Mohammad Taghi Kheirkhah
Mehran Mokarrami
Danilo Garcia
Does auditory attentional bias determine craving for methamphetamine? A pilot study using a word recognition dichotic listening task
Heliyon
Methamphetamine craving
Implicit assessment
Attentional bias
Dichotic listening paradigm
title Does auditory attentional bias determine craving for methamphetamine? A pilot study using a word recognition dichotic listening task
title_full Does auditory attentional bias determine craving for methamphetamine? A pilot study using a word recognition dichotic listening task
title_fullStr Does auditory attentional bias determine craving for methamphetamine? A pilot study using a word recognition dichotic listening task
title_full_unstemmed Does auditory attentional bias determine craving for methamphetamine? A pilot study using a word recognition dichotic listening task
title_short Does auditory attentional bias determine craving for methamphetamine? A pilot study using a word recognition dichotic listening task
title_sort does auditory attentional bias determine craving for methamphetamine a pilot study using a word recognition dichotic listening task
topic Methamphetamine craving
Implicit assessment
Attentional bias
Dichotic listening paradigm
url http://www.sciencedirect.com/science/article/pii/S2405844022025993
work_keys_str_mv AT maryamkazemitabar doesauditoryattentionalbiasdeterminecravingformethamphetamineapilotstudyusingawordrecognitiondichoticlisteningtask
AT mohammadtaghikheirkhah doesauditoryattentionalbiasdeterminecravingformethamphetamineapilotstudyusingawordrecognitiondichoticlisteningtask
AT mehranmokarrami doesauditoryattentionalbiasdeterminecravingformethamphetamineapilotstudyusingawordrecognitiondichoticlisteningtask
AT danilogarcia doesauditoryattentionalbiasdeterminecravingformethamphetamineapilotstudyusingawordrecognitiondichoticlisteningtask