Listening to yourself is special: Evidence from global speech rate tracking.
Listeners are known to use adjacent contextual speech rate in processing temporally ambiguous speech sounds. For instance, an ambiguous vowel between short /α/ and long /a:/ in Dutch sounds relatively long (i.e., as /a:/) embedded in a fast precursor sentence, but short in a slow sentence. Besides t...
Main Authors: | , , |
---|---|
Format: | Article |
Language: | English |
Published: |
Public Library of Science (PLoS)
2018-01-01
|
Series: | PLoS ONE |
Online Access: | http://europepmc.org/articles/PMC6124796?pdf=render |
_version_ | 1818042290992578560 |
---|---|
author | Merel Maslowski Antje S Meyer Hans Rutger Bosker |
author_facet | Merel Maslowski Antje S Meyer Hans Rutger Bosker |
author_sort | Merel Maslowski |
collection | DOAJ |
description | Listeners are known to use adjacent contextual speech rate in processing temporally ambiguous speech sounds. For instance, an ambiguous vowel between short /α/ and long /a:/ in Dutch sounds relatively long (i.e., as /a:/) embedded in a fast precursor sentence, but short in a slow sentence. Besides the local speech rate, listeners also track talker-specific global speech rates. However, it is yet unclear whether other talkers' global rates are encoded with reference to a listener's self-produced rate. Three experiments addressed this question. In Experiment 1, one group of participants was instructed to speak fast, whereas another group had to speak slowly. The groups were compared on their perception of ambiguous /α/-/a:/ vowels embedded in neutral rate speech from another talker. In Experiment 2, the same participants listened to playback of their own speech and again evaluated target vowels in neutral rate speech. Neither of these experiments provided support for the involvement of self-produced speech in perception of another talker's speech rate. Experiment 3 repeated Experiment 2 but with a new participant sample that was unfamiliar with the participants from Experiment 2. This experiment revealed fewer /a:/ responses in neutral speech in the group also listening to a fast rate, suggesting that neutral speech sounds slow in the presence of a fast talker and vice versa. Taken together, the findings show that self-produced speech is processed differently from speech produced by others. They carry implications for our understanding of rate-dependent speech perception in dialogue settings, suggesting that both perceptual and cognitive mechanisms are involved. |
first_indexed | 2024-12-10T08:43:59Z |
format | Article |
id | doaj.art-670cdcd15b9949a5a7c59f2dc04b31af |
institution | Directory Open Access Journal |
issn | 1932-6203 |
language | English |
last_indexed | 2024-12-10T08:43:59Z |
publishDate | 2018-01-01 |
publisher | Public Library of Science (PLoS) |
record_format | Article |
series | PLoS ONE |
spelling | doaj.art-670cdcd15b9949a5a7c59f2dc04b31af2022-12-22T01:55:47ZengPublic Library of Science (PLoS)PLoS ONE1932-62032018-01-01139e020357110.1371/journal.pone.0203571Listening to yourself is special: Evidence from global speech rate tracking.Merel MaslowskiAntje S MeyerHans Rutger BoskerListeners are known to use adjacent contextual speech rate in processing temporally ambiguous speech sounds. For instance, an ambiguous vowel between short /α/ and long /a:/ in Dutch sounds relatively long (i.e., as /a:/) embedded in a fast precursor sentence, but short in a slow sentence. Besides the local speech rate, listeners also track talker-specific global speech rates. However, it is yet unclear whether other talkers' global rates are encoded with reference to a listener's self-produced rate. Three experiments addressed this question. In Experiment 1, one group of participants was instructed to speak fast, whereas another group had to speak slowly. The groups were compared on their perception of ambiguous /α/-/a:/ vowels embedded in neutral rate speech from another talker. In Experiment 2, the same participants listened to playback of their own speech and again evaluated target vowels in neutral rate speech. Neither of these experiments provided support for the involvement of self-produced speech in perception of another talker's speech rate. Experiment 3 repeated Experiment 2 but with a new participant sample that was unfamiliar with the participants from Experiment 2. This experiment revealed fewer /a:/ responses in neutral speech in the group also listening to a fast rate, suggesting that neutral speech sounds slow in the presence of a fast talker and vice versa. Taken together, the findings show that self-produced speech is processed differently from speech produced by others. They carry implications for our understanding of rate-dependent speech perception in dialogue settings, suggesting that both perceptual and cognitive mechanisms are involved.http://europepmc.org/articles/PMC6124796?pdf=render |
spellingShingle | Merel Maslowski Antje S Meyer Hans Rutger Bosker Listening to yourself is special: Evidence from global speech rate tracking. PLoS ONE |
title | Listening to yourself is special: Evidence from global speech rate tracking. |
title_full | Listening to yourself is special: Evidence from global speech rate tracking. |
title_fullStr | Listening to yourself is special: Evidence from global speech rate tracking. |
title_full_unstemmed | Listening to yourself is special: Evidence from global speech rate tracking. |
title_short | Listening to yourself is special: Evidence from global speech rate tracking. |
title_sort | listening to yourself is special evidence from global speech rate tracking |
url | http://europepmc.org/articles/PMC6124796?pdf=render |
work_keys_str_mv | AT merelmaslowski listeningtoyourselfisspecialevidencefromglobalspeechratetracking AT antjesmeyer listeningtoyourselfisspecialevidencefromglobalspeechratetracking AT hansrutgerbosker listeningtoyourselfisspecialevidencefromglobalspeechratetracking |