Eyespot configuration and predator approach direction affect the antipredator efficacy of eyespots

Many prey species possess eyespots: paired markings that often consist of two or more concentric circles. Predators are wary of such prey because eyespots are conspicuous and/or mistaken for vertebrate eyes. Here we used naïve domestic chicks as predators of artificial moth-like prey to test the hyp...

Full description

Bibliographic Details
Main Authors: John Skelhorn, Hannah M. Rowland
Format: Article
Language:English
Published: Frontiers Media S.A. 2022-10-01
Series:Frontiers in Ecology and Evolution
Subjects:
Online Access:https://www.frontiersin.org/articles/10.3389/fevo.2022.951967/full
_version_ 1817986276395057152
author John Skelhorn
John Skelhorn
Hannah M. Rowland
author_facet John Skelhorn
John Skelhorn
Hannah M. Rowland
author_sort John Skelhorn
collection DOAJ
description Many prey species possess eyespots: paired markings that often consist of two or more concentric circles. Predators are wary of such prey because eyespots are conspicuous and/or mistaken for vertebrate eyes. Here we used naïve domestic chicks as predators of artificial moth-like prey to test the hypothesis that both eyespots configuration and predator approach direction affect the antipredator efficacy of eyespots. We found that when chicks approached prey straight on, eyespots configuration did not influence attack latency. Chicks that approached from either the left or the right, were slower to attack prey in which the central circle of the eyespot was centrally placed or shifted in the direction of the chick’s approach, compared to prey in which the central circle had been shifted away from the direction of approach. These findings suggest that eyespots composed of concentric circles may protect prey against predators approaching from a wider range of directions than eyespots composed of eccentric circles. They are also consistent with the idea that eyespots are mistaken for eyes, and are perceived to pose a lesser risk when their “gaze” is averted from the approaching predator.
first_indexed 2024-04-14T00:06:55Z
format Article
id doaj.art-922186a53ff240a6b933a0f805f70404
institution Directory Open Access Journal
issn 2296-701X
language English
last_indexed 2024-04-14T00:06:55Z
publishDate 2022-10-01
publisher Frontiers Media S.A.
record_format Article
series Frontiers in Ecology and Evolution
spelling doaj.art-922186a53ff240a6b933a0f805f704042022-12-22T02:23:29ZengFrontiers Media S.A.Frontiers in Ecology and Evolution2296-701X2022-10-011010.3389/fevo.2022.951967951967Eyespot configuration and predator approach direction affect the antipredator efficacy of eyespotsJohn Skelhorn0John Skelhorn1Hannah M. Rowland2Faculty of Medical Sciences, Biosciences Institute, Newcastle University, Newcastle upon Tyne, United KingdomCentre for Research in Animal Behaviour, College of Life and Environmental Sciences, University of Exeter, Washington Singer Laboratories, Exeter, United KingdomMax Planck Institute for Chemical Ecology, Jena, GermanyMany prey species possess eyespots: paired markings that often consist of two or more concentric circles. Predators are wary of such prey because eyespots are conspicuous and/or mistaken for vertebrate eyes. Here we used naïve domestic chicks as predators of artificial moth-like prey to test the hypothesis that both eyespots configuration and predator approach direction affect the antipredator efficacy of eyespots. We found that when chicks approached prey straight on, eyespots configuration did not influence attack latency. Chicks that approached from either the left or the right, were slower to attack prey in which the central circle of the eyespot was centrally placed or shifted in the direction of the chick’s approach, compared to prey in which the central circle had been shifted away from the direction of approach. These findings suggest that eyespots composed of concentric circles may protect prey against predators approaching from a wider range of directions than eyespots composed of eccentric circles. They are also consistent with the idea that eyespots are mistaken for eyes, and are perceived to pose a lesser risk when their “gaze” is averted from the approaching predator.https://www.frontiersin.org/articles/10.3389/fevo.2022.951967/fullantipredator defenseeye gazemimicrypredator-preyprotective colorationwariness
spellingShingle John Skelhorn
John Skelhorn
Hannah M. Rowland
Eyespot configuration and predator approach direction affect the antipredator efficacy of eyespots
Frontiers in Ecology and Evolution
antipredator defense
eye gaze
mimicry
predator-prey
protective coloration
wariness
title Eyespot configuration and predator approach direction affect the antipredator efficacy of eyespots
title_full Eyespot configuration and predator approach direction affect the antipredator efficacy of eyespots
title_fullStr Eyespot configuration and predator approach direction affect the antipredator efficacy of eyespots
title_full_unstemmed Eyespot configuration and predator approach direction affect the antipredator efficacy of eyespots
title_short Eyespot configuration and predator approach direction affect the antipredator efficacy of eyespots
title_sort eyespot configuration and predator approach direction affect the antipredator efficacy of eyespots
topic antipredator defense
eye gaze
mimicry
predator-prey
protective coloration
wariness
url https://www.frontiersin.org/articles/10.3389/fevo.2022.951967/full
work_keys_str_mv AT johnskelhorn eyespotconfigurationandpredatorapproachdirectionaffecttheantipredatorefficacyofeyespots
AT johnskelhorn eyespotconfigurationandpredatorapproachdirectionaffecttheantipredatorefficacyofeyespots
AT hannahmrowland eyespotconfigurationandpredatorapproachdirectionaffecttheantipredatorefficacyofeyespots