Still no compelling evidence that Americans overestimate upward socio-economic mobility rates: Reply to Davidai and Gilovich (2018)

Davidai and Gilovich (2018) contend that (a) Americans tend to think about their nation's income distribution in terms of quintiles (fifths), and (b) when Americans' perceptions of socio-economic mobility rates are measured properly (e.g., by asking online survey respondents to guess upwar...

Full description

Bibliographic Details
Main Authors: Sondre S. Nero, Lawton K. Swan, John R. Chambers
Format: Article
Language:English
Published: Cambridge University Press 2018-05-01
Series:Judgment and Decision Making
Subjects:
Online Access:http://journal.sjdm.org/17/17911br/jdm17911br.pdf
_version_ 1797706731843747840
author Sondre S. Nero
Lawton K. Swan
John R. Chambers
author_facet Sondre S. Nero
Lawton K. Swan
John R. Chambers
author_sort Sondre S. Nero
collection DOAJ
description Davidai and Gilovich (2018) contend that (a) Americans tend to think about their nation's income distribution in terms of quintiles (fifths), and (b) when Americans' perceptions of socio-economic mobility rates are measured properly (e.g., by asking online survey respondents to guess upward-mobility rates across quintiles), a trend of overestimation (too much optimism concerning the number of people who manage to transcend poverty) will emerge. In this reply, we hail Davidai and Gilovich's new data as novel, important, and relevant to the former (a), but we doubt that they can support the latter (b) claim about population-level (in)accuracy. Namely, we note that even if mobility-rate perceptions could be measured perfectly, inferences about the accuracy of those perceptions still depend on a particular comparator—a point-estimate of the "true" rate of upward social mobility in the U.S. against which survey respondents' guesses are evaluated—that is itself an error-prone estimate. Applying different established comparators to survey respondents' guesses changes both the direction and magnitude of previously observed overestimation effects. We conclude with a challenge: researchers who wish to compute the average distance between socio-economic perceptions and socio-economic reality must first select and justify a fair comparator.
first_indexed 2024-03-12T05:55:41Z
format Article
id doaj.art-9ad1ebc0a995409d9580e9fe0af3abcf
institution Directory Open Access Journal
issn 1930-2975
language English
last_indexed 2024-03-12T05:55:41Z
publishDate 2018-05-01
publisher Cambridge University Press
record_format Article
series Judgment and Decision Making
spelling doaj.art-9ad1ebc0a995409d9580e9fe0af3abcf2023-09-03T04:37:25ZengCambridge University PressJudgment and Decision Making1930-29752018-05-01133305308Still no compelling evidence that Americans overestimate upward socio-economic mobility rates: Reply to Davidai and Gilovich (2018)Sondre S. NeroLawton K. SwanJohn R. ChambersDavidai and Gilovich (2018) contend that (a) Americans tend to think about their nation's income distribution in terms of quintiles (fifths), and (b) when Americans' perceptions of socio-economic mobility rates are measured properly (e.g., by asking online survey respondents to guess upward-mobility rates across quintiles), a trend of overestimation (too much optimism concerning the number of people who manage to transcend poverty) will emerge. In this reply, we hail Davidai and Gilovich's new data as novel, important, and relevant to the former (a), but we doubt that they can support the latter (b) claim about population-level (in)accuracy. Namely, we note that even if mobility-rate perceptions could be measured perfectly, inferences about the accuracy of those perceptions still depend on a particular comparator—a point-estimate of the "true" rate of upward social mobility in the U.S. against which survey respondents' guesses are evaluated—that is itself an error-prone estimate. Applying different established comparators to survey respondents' guesses changes both the direction and magnitude of previously observed overestimation effects. We conclude with a challenge: researchers who wish to compute the average distance between socio-economic perceptions and socio-economic reality must first select and justify a fair comparator.http://journal.sjdm.org/17/17911br/jdm17911br.pdfsocial mobility inequality political ideology lay beliefs.NAKeywords
spellingShingle Sondre S. Nero
Lawton K. Swan
John R. Chambers
Still no compelling evidence that Americans overestimate upward socio-economic mobility rates: Reply to Davidai and Gilovich (2018)
Judgment and Decision Making
social mobility
inequality
political ideology
lay beliefs.NAKeywords
title Still no compelling evidence that Americans overestimate upward socio-economic mobility rates: Reply to Davidai and Gilovich (2018)
title_full Still no compelling evidence that Americans overestimate upward socio-economic mobility rates: Reply to Davidai and Gilovich (2018)
title_fullStr Still no compelling evidence that Americans overestimate upward socio-economic mobility rates: Reply to Davidai and Gilovich (2018)
title_full_unstemmed Still no compelling evidence that Americans overestimate upward socio-economic mobility rates: Reply to Davidai and Gilovich (2018)
title_short Still no compelling evidence that Americans overestimate upward socio-economic mobility rates: Reply to Davidai and Gilovich (2018)
title_sort still no compelling evidence that americans overestimate upward socio economic mobility rates reply to davidai and gilovich 2018
topic social mobility
inequality
political ideology
lay beliefs.NAKeywords
url http://journal.sjdm.org/17/17911br/jdm17911br.pdf
work_keys_str_mv AT sondresnero stillnocompellingevidencethatamericansoverestimateupwardsocioeconomicmobilityratesreplytodavidaiandgilovich2018
AT lawtonkswan stillnocompellingevidencethatamericansoverestimateupwardsocioeconomicmobilityratesreplytodavidaiandgilovich2018
AT johnrchambers stillnocompellingevidencethatamericansoverestimateupwardsocioeconomicmobilityratesreplytodavidaiandgilovich2018