Distinguishing target from distractor in Stroop, picture-word, and word-word interference tasks

Lexical selection – both during reading aloud and speech production – involves selecting an intended word, while ignoring irrelevant lexical activation. This process has been studied by the use of interference tasks. Examples are the Stroop task, where participants ignore the written color word and...

Full description

Bibliographic Details
Main Authors: Xenia eSchmalz, Barbara eTreccani, Claudio eMulatti
Format: Article
Language:English
Published: Frontiers Media S.A. 2015-12-01
Series:Frontiers in Psychology
Subjects:
Online Access:http://journal.frontiersin.org/Journal/10.3389/fpsyg.2015.01858/full
_version_ 1818972255595528192
author Xenia eSchmalz
Xenia eSchmalz
Barbara eTreccani
Claudio eMulatti
author_facet Xenia eSchmalz
Xenia eSchmalz
Barbara eTreccani
Claudio eMulatti
author_sort Xenia eSchmalz
collection DOAJ
description Lexical selection – both during reading aloud and speech production – involves selecting an intended word, while ignoring irrelevant lexical activation. This process has been studied by the use of interference tasks. Examples are the Stroop task, where participants ignore the written color word and name the color of the ink, picture-word interference tasks, where participants name a picture while ignoring a super-imposed written word, or word-word interference (WWI) tasks, where two words are presented and the participants need to respond to only one, based on an pre-determined visual feature (e.g., color, position). Here, we focus on the WWI task: it is theoretically impossible for existing models to explain how the cognitive system can respond to one stimulus and block the other, when they are presented by the same modality (i.e., they are both words). We describe a solution that can explain performance on the WWI task: drawing on the literature on visual attention, we propose that the system creates an object file for each perceived object, which is continuously updated with increasingly complete information about the stimulus, such as the task-relevant visual feature. Such a model can account for performance on all three tasks.
first_indexed 2024-12-20T15:05:22Z
format Article
id doaj.art-a0b0e344e22e4408939907abc6109b92
institution Directory Open Access Journal
issn 1664-1078
language English
last_indexed 2024-12-20T15:05:22Z
publishDate 2015-12-01
publisher Frontiers Media S.A.
record_format Article
series Frontiers in Psychology
spelling doaj.art-a0b0e344e22e4408939907abc6109b922022-12-21T19:36:32ZengFrontiers Media S.A.Frontiers in Psychology1664-10782015-12-01610.3389/fpsyg.2015.01858153148Distinguishing target from distractor in Stroop, picture-word, and word-word interference tasksXenia eSchmalz0Xenia eSchmalz1Barbara eTreccani2Claudio eMulatti3Università degli Studi di PadovaMacquarie UniversityUniversità degli Studi di SassariUniversità degli Studi di PadovaLexical selection – both during reading aloud and speech production – involves selecting an intended word, while ignoring irrelevant lexical activation. This process has been studied by the use of interference tasks. Examples are the Stroop task, where participants ignore the written color word and name the color of the ink, picture-word interference tasks, where participants name a picture while ignoring a super-imposed written word, or word-word interference (WWI) tasks, where two words are presented and the participants need to respond to only one, based on an pre-determined visual feature (e.g., color, position). Here, we focus on the WWI task: it is theoretically impossible for existing models to explain how the cognitive system can respond to one stimulus and block the other, when they are presented by the same modality (i.e., they are both words). We describe a solution that can explain performance on the WWI task: drawing on the literature on visual attention, we propose that the system creates an object file for each perceived object, which is continuously updated with increasingly complete information about the stimulus, such as the task-relevant visual feature. Such a model can account for performance on all three tasks.http://journal.frontiersin.org/Journal/10.3389/fpsyg.2015.01858/fullStroop Testselective attentionmental Lexiconpicture-word interferenceLexical selection by competitionWord-word interference
spellingShingle Xenia eSchmalz
Xenia eSchmalz
Barbara eTreccani
Claudio eMulatti
Distinguishing target from distractor in Stroop, picture-word, and word-word interference tasks
Frontiers in Psychology
Stroop Test
selective attention
mental Lexicon
picture-word interference
Lexical selection by competition
Word-word interference
title Distinguishing target from distractor in Stroop, picture-word, and word-word interference tasks
title_full Distinguishing target from distractor in Stroop, picture-word, and word-word interference tasks
title_fullStr Distinguishing target from distractor in Stroop, picture-word, and word-word interference tasks
title_full_unstemmed Distinguishing target from distractor in Stroop, picture-word, and word-word interference tasks
title_short Distinguishing target from distractor in Stroop, picture-word, and word-word interference tasks
title_sort distinguishing target from distractor in stroop picture word and word word interference tasks
topic Stroop Test
selective attention
mental Lexicon
picture-word interference
Lexical selection by competition
Word-word interference
url http://journal.frontiersin.org/Journal/10.3389/fpsyg.2015.01858/full
work_keys_str_mv AT xeniaeschmalz distinguishingtargetfromdistractorinstrooppicturewordandwordwordinterferencetasks
AT xeniaeschmalz distinguishingtargetfromdistractorinstrooppicturewordandwordwordinterferencetasks
AT barbaraetreccani distinguishingtargetfromdistractorinstrooppicturewordandwordwordinterferencetasks
AT claudioemulatti distinguishingtargetfromdistractorinstrooppicturewordandwordwordinterferencetasks