Is Your Color My Color? Dividing the Labor of the Stroop Task Between Co-actors
Performing a task with other actors involves two opposing forces, division of labor between co-acting individuals and integration of divided parts of the task into a shared mental representation (co-representation). Previous studies have focused primarily on the integration of task representations a...
Main Authors: | , , |
---|---|
Format: | Article |
Language: | English |
Published: |
Frontiers Media S.A.
2018-08-01
|
Series: | Frontiers in Psychology |
Subjects: | |
Online Access: | https://www.frontiersin.org/article/10.3389/fpsyg.2018.01407/full |
_version_ | 1819040984103649280 |
---|---|
author | Motonori Yamaguchi Emma L. Clarke Danny L. Egan |
author_facet | Motonori Yamaguchi Emma L. Clarke Danny L. Egan |
author_sort | Motonori Yamaguchi |
collection | DOAJ |
description | Performing a task with other actors involves two opposing forces, division of labor between co-acting individuals and integration of divided parts of the task into a shared mental representation (co-representation). Previous studies have focused primarily on the integration of task representations and limited attention has paid to the division of labor. The present study devised a test of the integration and the division in a joint task setting. A joint version of the Stroop task was developed, in which pairs of actors were assigned different sets of target colors. If the actors integrate their co-actor’s task, the colors assigned to their co-actor should be represented as if they were the actor’s own target colors; the Stroop effect should be as large when distractor color words denote their co-actor’s target colors as when these words denote the actor’s own target colors. If the actors divide the labor of the Stroop task, the colors assigned to their partner should be represented as non-target colors; the Stroop effect should be smaller when the distractor color words denote the co-actor’s target colors than when these words denote the actor’s own target colors. The results of response time did not provide clear support for either position, while those of response accuracy supported the division of labor. Possible cognitive mechanisms that support the division of labor and the integration of task representation are discussed. |
first_indexed | 2024-12-21T09:17:47Z |
format | Article |
id | doaj.art-a7ec5fa32d8a42689780dbc85e046f08 |
institution | Directory Open Access Journal |
issn | 1664-1078 |
language | English |
last_indexed | 2024-12-21T09:17:47Z |
publishDate | 2018-08-01 |
publisher | Frontiers Media S.A. |
record_format | Article |
series | Frontiers in Psychology |
spelling | doaj.art-a7ec5fa32d8a42689780dbc85e046f082022-12-21T19:09:07ZengFrontiers Media S.A.Frontiers in Psychology1664-10782018-08-01910.3389/fpsyg.2018.01407331744Is Your Color My Color? Dividing the Labor of the Stroop Task Between Co-actorsMotonori YamaguchiEmma L. ClarkeDanny L. EganPerforming a task with other actors involves two opposing forces, division of labor between co-acting individuals and integration of divided parts of the task into a shared mental representation (co-representation). Previous studies have focused primarily on the integration of task representations and limited attention has paid to the division of labor. The present study devised a test of the integration and the division in a joint task setting. A joint version of the Stroop task was developed, in which pairs of actors were assigned different sets of target colors. If the actors integrate their co-actor’s task, the colors assigned to their co-actor should be represented as if they were the actor’s own target colors; the Stroop effect should be as large when distractor color words denote their co-actor’s target colors as when these words denote the actor’s own target colors. If the actors divide the labor of the Stroop task, the colors assigned to their partner should be represented as non-target colors; the Stroop effect should be smaller when the distractor color words denote the co-actor’s target colors than when these words denote the actor’s own target colors. The results of response time did not provide clear support for either position, while those of response accuracy supported the division of labor. Possible cognitive mechanisms that support the division of labor and the integration of task representation are discussed.https://www.frontiersin.org/article/10.3389/fpsyg.2018.01407/fulljoint performanceStroop interferencesemantic gradientdivision of laborco-representation |
spellingShingle | Motonori Yamaguchi Emma L. Clarke Danny L. Egan Is Your Color My Color? Dividing the Labor of the Stroop Task Between Co-actors Frontiers in Psychology joint performance Stroop interference semantic gradient division of labor co-representation |
title | Is Your Color My Color? Dividing the Labor of the Stroop Task Between Co-actors |
title_full | Is Your Color My Color? Dividing the Labor of the Stroop Task Between Co-actors |
title_fullStr | Is Your Color My Color? Dividing the Labor of the Stroop Task Between Co-actors |
title_full_unstemmed | Is Your Color My Color? Dividing the Labor of the Stroop Task Between Co-actors |
title_short | Is Your Color My Color? Dividing the Labor of the Stroop Task Between Co-actors |
title_sort | is your color my color dividing the labor of the stroop task between co actors |
topic | joint performance Stroop interference semantic gradient division of labor co-representation |
url | https://www.frontiersin.org/article/10.3389/fpsyg.2018.01407/full |
work_keys_str_mv | AT motonoriyamaguchi isyourcolormycolordividingthelaborofthestrooptaskbetweencoactors AT emmalclarke isyourcolormycolordividingthelaborofthestrooptaskbetweencoactors AT dannylegan isyourcolormycolordividingthelaborofthestrooptaskbetweencoactors |