Unlocking the Power of Gesture: Using Movement-Based Instruction to Improve First Grade Children’s Spatial Unit Misconceptions

Gestures are hand movements that are produced simultaneously with spoken language and can supplement it by representing semantic information, emphasizing important points, or showing spatial locations and relations. Gestures’ specific features make them a promising tool to improve spatial thinking....

Full description

Bibliographic Details
Main Authors: Eliza L. Congdon, Susan C. Levine
Format: Article
Language:English
Published: MDPI AG 2023-10-01
Series:Journal of Intelligence
Subjects:
Online Access:https://www.mdpi.com/2079-3200/11/10/200
_version_ 1797573405073997824
author Eliza L. Congdon
Susan C. Levine
author_facet Eliza L. Congdon
Susan C. Levine
author_sort Eliza L. Congdon
collection DOAJ
description Gestures are hand movements that are produced simultaneously with spoken language and can supplement it by representing semantic information, emphasizing important points, or showing spatial locations and relations. Gestures’ specific features make them a promising tool to improve spatial thinking. Yet, there is recent work showing that not all learners benefit equally from gesture instruction and that this may be driven, in part, by children’s difficulty understanding what an instructor’s gesture is intended to represent. The current study directly compares instruction with gestures to instruction with plastic unit chips (Action) in a linear measurement learning paradigm aimed at teaching children the concept of spatial units. Some children performed only one type of movement, and some children performed both: Action-then-Gesture [AG] or Gesture-then-Action [GA]. Children learned most from the Gesture-then-Action [GA] and Action only [A] training conditions. After controlling for initial differences in learning, the gesture-then-action condition outperformed all three other training conditions on a transfer task. While gesture is cognitively challenging for some learners, that challenge may be desirable—immediately following gesture with a concrete representation to clarify that gesture’s meaning is an especially effective way to unlock the power of this spatial tool and lead to deep, generalizable learning.
first_indexed 2024-03-10T21:09:12Z
format Article
id doaj.art-c05211eeb9b349ba9cff658fb4c54918
institution Directory Open Access Journal
issn 2079-3200
language English
last_indexed 2024-03-10T21:09:12Z
publishDate 2023-10-01
publisher MDPI AG
record_format Article
series Journal of Intelligence
spelling doaj.art-c05211eeb9b349ba9cff658fb4c549182023-11-19T16:56:53ZengMDPI AGJournal of Intelligence2079-32002023-10-01111020010.3390/jintelligence11100200Unlocking the Power of Gesture: Using Movement-Based Instruction to Improve First Grade Children’s Spatial Unit MisconceptionsEliza L. Congdon0Susan C. Levine1Department of Psychology, Williams College, Williamstown, MA 01267, USADepartment of Psychology, The University of Chicago, Chicago, IL 60637, USAGestures are hand movements that are produced simultaneously with spoken language and can supplement it by representing semantic information, emphasizing important points, or showing spatial locations and relations. Gestures’ specific features make them a promising tool to improve spatial thinking. Yet, there is recent work showing that not all learners benefit equally from gesture instruction and that this may be driven, in part, by children’s difficulty understanding what an instructor’s gesture is intended to represent. The current study directly compares instruction with gestures to instruction with plastic unit chips (Action) in a linear measurement learning paradigm aimed at teaching children the concept of spatial units. Some children performed only one type of movement, and some children performed both: Action-then-Gesture [AG] or Gesture-then-Action [GA]. Children learned most from the Gesture-then-Action [GA] and Action only [A] training conditions. After controlling for initial differences in learning, the gesture-then-action condition outperformed all three other training conditions on a transfer task. While gesture is cognitively challenging for some learners, that challenge may be desirable—immediately following gesture with a concrete representation to clarify that gesture’s meaning is an especially effective way to unlock the power of this spatial tool and lead to deep, generalizable learning.https://www.mdpi.com/2079-3200/11/10/200gesturespatial thinkingunitslinear measurementdesirable difficulty
spellingShingle Eliza L. Congdon
Susan C. Levine
Unlocking the Power of Gesture: Using Movement-Based Instruction to Improve First Grade Children’s Spatial Unit Misconceptions
Journal of Intelligence
gesture
spatial thinking
units
linear measurement
desirable difficulty
title Unlocking the Power of Gesture: Using Movement-Based Instruction to Improve First Grade Children’s Spatial Unit Misconceptions
title_full Unlocking the Power of Gesture: Using Movement-Based Instruction to Improve First Grade Children’s Spatial Unit Misconceptions
title_fullStr Unlocking the Power of Gesture: Using Movement-Based Instruction to Improve First Grade Children’s Spatial Unit Misconceptions
title_full_unstemmed Unlocking the Power of Gesture: Using Movement-Based Instruction to Improve First Grade Children’s Spatial Unit Misconceptions
title_short Unlocking the Power of Gesture: Using Movement-Based Instruction to Improve First Grade Children’s Spatial Unit Misconceptions
title_sort unlocking the power of gesture using movement based instruction to improve first grade children s spatial unit misconceptions
topic gesture
spatial thinking
units
linear measurement
desirable difficulty
url https://www.mdpi.com/2079-3200/11/10/200
work_keys_str_mv AT elizalcongdon unlockingthepowerofgestureusingmovementbasedinstructiontoimprovefirstgradechildrensspatialunitmisconceptions
AT susanclevine unlockingthepowerofgestureusingmovementbasedinstructiontoimprovefirstgradechildrensspatialunitmisconceptions