A place to hide in the home-cage decreases yolk androgen levels and offspring emotional reactivity in Japanese quail.

An animal's emotional responses are the result of its cognitive appraisal of a situation. This appraisal is notably influenced by the possibility of an individual to exert control over an aversive event. Although the fact that environment controllability decreases emotional responses in animals...

Full description

Bibliographic Details
Main Authors: Vanessa Guesdon, Aline Bertin, Cécilia Houdelier, Sophie Lumineau, Laureline Formanek, Kurt Kotrschal, Erich Möstl, Marie-Annick Richard-Yris
Format: Article
Language:English
Published: Public Library of Science (PLoS) 2011-01-01
Series:PLoS ONE
Online Access:http://europepmc.org/articles/PMC3182999?pdf=render
_version_ 1819092066888581120
author Vanessa Guesdon
Aline Bertin
Cécilia Houdelier
Sophie Lumineau
Laureline Formanek
Kurt Kotrschal
Erich Möstl
Marie-Annick Richard-Yris
author_facet Vanessa Guesdon
Aline Bertin
Cécilia Houdelier
Sophie Lumineau
Laureline Formanek
Kurt Kotrschal
Erich Möstl
Marie-Annick Richard-Yris
author_sort Vanessa Guesdon
collection DOAJ
description An animal's emotional responses are the result of its cognitive appraisal of a situation. This appraisal is notably influenced by the possibility of an individual to exert control over an aversive event. Although the fact that environment controllability decreases emotional responses in animals is well established, far less is known about its potential trans-generational effects. As the levels of avian yolk hormones can vary according to the mother's environment, we hypothesized that housing environment of mothers would modulate the quality of her eggs and in turn her offspring's behaviour. Two groups of female Japanese quail were constituted: a group that had access to a place to hide in their home-cage (Hd, n = 20) and a group that had nowhere to hide (NoHd, n = 20) when stressed. Both groups were submitted to daily human disturbances for a twenty-day-period. Hd females produced eggs with both less testosterone and androstenedione than did NoHd females. The emotional and social reactivity of Hd females' offspring were lower and their growth was slower than those of NoHd females' offspring. Our results show that a minor difference in housing environment had substantial effects on eggs and offspring. The presence of a shelter probably helped quail to cope with daily human disturbances, producing less reactive offspring. This transgenerational effect caused by an opportunity to hide could lead to applications in care of laboratory animals, conservation biology and animal welfare.
first_indexed 2024-12-21T22:49:43Z
format Article
id doaj.art-c524bf1e80df4e8f9c223b2358df3100
institution Directory Open Access Journal
issn 1932-6203
language English
last_indexed 2024-12-21T22:49:43Z
publishDate 2011-01-01
publisher Public Library of Science (PLoS)
record_format Article
series PLoS ONE
spelling doaj.art-c524bf1e80df4e8f9c223b2358df31002022-12-21T18:47:37ZengPublic Library of Science (PLoS)PLoS ONE1932-62032011-01-0169e2394110.1371/journal.pone.0023941A place to hide in the home-cage decreases yolk androgen levels and offspring emotional reactivity in Japanese quail.Vanessa GuesdonAline BertinCécilia HoudelierSophie LumineauLaureline FormanekKurt KotrschalErich MöstlMarie-Annick Richard-YrisAn animal's emotional responses are the result of its cognitive appraisal of a situation. This appraisal is notably influenced by the possibility of an individual to exert control over an aversive event. Although the fact that environment controllability decreases emotional responses in animals is well established, far less is known about its potential trans-generational effects. As the levels of avian yolk hormones can vary according to the mother's environment, we hypothesized that housing environment of mothers would modulate the quality of her eggs and in turn her offspring's behaviour. Two groups of female Japanese quail were constituted: a group that had access to a place to hide in their home-cage (Hd, n = 20) and a group that had nowhere to hide (NoHd, n = 20) when stressed. Both groups were submitted to daily human disturbances for a twenty-day-period. Hd females produced eggs with both less testosterone and androstenedione than did NoHd females. The emotional and social reactivity of Hd females' offspring were lower and their growth was slower than those of NoHd females' offspring. Our results show that a minor difference in housing environment had substantial effects on eggs and offspring. The presence of a shelter probably helped quail to cope with daily human disturbances, producing less reactive offspring. This transgenerational effect caused by an opportunity to hide could lead to applications in care of laboratory animals, conservation biology and animal welfare.http://europepmc.org/articles/PMC3182999?pdf=render
spellingShingle Vanessa Guesdon
Aline Bertin
Cécilia Houdelier
Sophie Lumineau
Laureline Formanek
Kurt Kotrschal
Erich Möstl
Marie-Annick Richard-Yris
A place to hide in the home-cage decreases yolk androgen levels and offspring emotional reactivity in Japanese quail.
PLoS ONE
title A place to hide in the home-cage decreases yolk androgen levels and offspring emotional reactivity in Japanese quail.
title_full A place to hide in the home-cage decreases yolk androgen levels and offspring emotional reactivity in Japanese quail.
title_fullStr A place to hide in the home-cage decreases yolk androgen levels and offspring emotional reactivity in Japanese quail.
title_full_unstemmed A place to hide in the home-cage decreases yolk androgen levels and offspring emotional reactivity in Japanese quail.
title_short A place to hide in the home-cage decreases yolk androgen levels and offspring emotional reactivity in Japanese quail.
title_sort place to hide in the home cage decreases yolk androgen levels and offspring emotional reactivity in japanese quail
url http://europepmc.org/articles/PMC3182999?pdf=render
work_keys_str_mv AT vanessaguesdon aplacetohideinthehomecagedecreasesyolkandrogenlevelsandoffspringemotionalreactivityinjapanesequail
AT alinebertin aplacetohideinthehomecagedecreasesyolkandrogenlevelsandoffspringemotionalreactivityinjapanesequail
AT ceciliahoudelier aplacetohideinthehomecagedecreasesyolkandrogenlevelsandoffspringemotionalreactivityinjapanesequail
AT sophielumineau aplacetohideinthehomecagedecreasesyolkandrogenlevelsandoffspringemotionalreactivityinjapanesequail
AT laurelineformanek aplacetohideinthehomecagedecreasesyolkandrogenlevelsandoffspringemotionalreactivityinjapanesequail
AT kurtkotrschal aplacetohideinthehomecagedecreasesyolkandrogenlevelsandoffspringemotionalreactivityinjapanesequail
AT erichmostl aplacetohideinthehomecagedecreasesyolkandrogenlevelsandoffspringemotionalreactivityinjapanesequail
AT marieannickrichardyris aplacetohideinthehomecagedecreasesyolkandrogenlevelsandoffspringemotionalreactivityinjapanesequail
AT vanessaguesdon placetohideinthehomecagedecreasesyolkandrogenlevelsandoffspringemotionalreactivityinjapanesequail
AT alinebertin placetohideinthehomecagedecreasesyolkandrogenlevelsandoffspringemotionalreactivityinjapanesequail
AT ceciliahoudelier placetohideinthehomecagedecreasesyolkandrogenlevelsandoffspringemotionalreactivityinjapanesequail
AT sophielumineau placetohideinthehomecagedecreasesyolkandrogenlevelsandoffspringemotionalreactivityinjapanesequail
AT laurelineformanek placetohideinthehomecagedecreasesyolkandrogenlevelsandoffspringemotionalreactivityinjapanesequail
AT kurtkotrschal placetohideinthehomecagedecreasesyolkandrogenlevelsandoffspringemotionalreactivityinjapanesequail
AT erichmostl placetohideinthehomecagedecreasesyolkandrogenlevelsandoffspringemotionalreactivityinjapanesequail
AT marieannickrichardyris placetohideinthehomecagedecreasesyolkandrogenlevelsandoffspringemotionalreactivityinjapanesequail