P2-4: What is Special about Action Video Games for Training Visual Cognition?

Previous training studies (e.g., Green and Bavelier, 2003 Nature 423 534–537) link action video games (AVG) to improvements in visual cognition. However, specific components of AVGs that result in these improvements remain undetermined. One possibility is that only near-transfer occurs because game-...

Full description

Bibliographic Details
Main Authors: Adam Oei, Michael Patterson
Format: Article
Language:English
Published: SAGE Publishing 2012-10-01
Series:i-Perception
Online Access:https://doi.org/10.1068/if665
_version_ 1819206391933435904
author Adam Oei
Michael Patterson
author_facet Adam Oei
Michael Patterson
author_sort Adam Oei
collection DOAJ
description Previous training studies (e.g., Green and Bavelier, 2003 Nature 423 534–537) link action video games (AVG) to improvements in visual cognition. However, specific components of AVGs that result in these improvements remain undetermined. One possibility is that only near-transfer occurs because game-play demands closely resemble the cognitive behaviors tested. Alternatively, far-transfer may occur to cognitive behaviors not specifically trained. To test between these possibilities, 4 groups of participants played 4 different AVG for 20 hours. Games differed in speed, number of simultaneous items to-be-tracked, and attentional switching demands. Group 1's AVG was similar to games in previous training studies with high speed and attentional demands, and multiple concurrent enemies (Modern Combat). Group 2's AVG had lower object-tracking demands due to sequential instead of concurrent enemies (Metal Gear Solid Touch). Group 3's AVG had slower speed by delaying counterattacks of enemies (Arcade Super Sniper). A fourth group played a hunting AVG that included target searching with no counterattacking enemies, and thus no speed, switching, or multiple object-tracking demands (Deer Hunter). Three tests of visual cognition—attentional blink, change detection, and visual search—were administered before and after training. Attentional blink was reduced only in groups 1 and 2 whose training contained high-speed and attentional switch demands. Only Group 1, with training of attending to multiple simultaneous attackers, improved change detection in multiple objects. All games contained visual search, and thus all groups improved in visual search after training. Results support the near-transfer proposal for visual cognition improvement.
first_indexed 2024-12-23T05:06:52Z
format Article
id doaj.art-d1179e01c3244e42ae50e10879334b4b
institution Directory Open Access Journal
issn 2041-6695
language English
last_indexed 2024-12-23T05:06:52Z
publishDate 2012-10-01
publisher SAGE Publishing
record_format Article
series i-Perception
spelling doaj.art-d1179e01c3244e42ae50e10879334b4b2022-12-21T17:59:04ZengSAGE Publishingi-Perception2041-66952012-10-01310.1068/if66510.1068_if665P2-4: What is Special about Action Video Games for Training Visual Cognition?Adam Oei0Michael PattersonNanyang Technological UniversityPrevious training studies (e.g., Green and Bavelier, 2003 Nature 423 534–537) link action video games (AVG) to improvements in visual cognition. However, specific components of AVGs that result in these improvements remain undetermined. One possibility is that only near-transfer occurs because game-play demands closely resemble the cognitive behaviors tested. Alternatively, far-transfer may occur to cognitive behaviors not specifically trained. To test between these possibilities, 4 groups of participants played 4 different AVG for 20 hours. Games differed in speed, number of simultaneous items to-be-tracked, and attentional switching demands. Group 1's AVG was similar to games in previous training studies with high speed and attentional demands, and multiple concurrent enemies (Modern Combat). Group 2's AVG had lower object-tracking demands due to sequential instead of concurrent enemies (Metal Gear Solid Touch). Group 3's AVG had slower speed by delaying counterattacks of enemies (Arcade Super Sniper). A fourth group played a hunting AVG that included target searching with no counterattacking enemies, and thus no speed, switching, or multiple object-tracking demands (Deer Hunter). Three tests of visual cognition—attentional blink, change detection, and visual search—were administered before and after training. Attentional blink was reduced only in groups 1 and 2 whose training contained high-speed and attentional switch demands. Only Group 1, with training of attending to multiple simultaneous attackers, improved change detection in multiple objects. All games contained visual search, and thus all groups improved in visual search after training. Results support the near-transfer proposal for visual cognition improvement.https://doi.org/10.1068/if665
spellingShingle Adam Oei
Michael Patterson
P2-4: What is Special about Action Video Games for Training Visual Cognition?
i-Perception
title P2-4: What is Special about Action Video Games for Training Visual Cognition?
title_full P2-4: What is Special about Action Video Games for Training Visual Cognition?
title_fullStr P2-4: What is Special about Action Video Games for Training Visual Cognition?
title_full_unstemmed P2-4: What is Special about Action Video Games for Training Visual Cognition?
title_short P2-4: What is Special about Action Video Games for Training Visual Cognition?
title_sort p2 4 what is special about action video games for training visual cognition
url https://doi.org/10.1068/if665
work_keys_str_mv AT adamoei p24whatisspecialaboutactionvideogamesfortrainingvisualcognition
AT michaelpatterson p24whatisspecialaboutactionvideogamesfortrainingvisualcognition