How does graphotactic knowledge influence children’s learning of new spellings?
Two experiments investigated whether and how the learning of spellings by French third graders is influenced by two graphotactic patterns: consonants cannot double in word-initial position (Experiment 1) and consonants cannot double after single consonants (Experiment 2). Children silently read mean...
Main Authors: | , , , |
---|---|
Format: | Article |
Language: | English |
Published: |
Frontiers Media S.A.
2013-10-01
|
Series: | Frontiers in Psychology |
Subjects: | |
Online Access: | http://journal.frontiersin.org/Journal/10.3389/fpsyg.2013.00701/full |
_version_ | 1819204789943140352 |
---|---|
author | Sebastien ePacton Amélie eSobaco Amélie eSobaco Michel eFayol Rebecca eTreiman |
author_facet | Sebastien ePacton Amélie eSobaco Amélie eSobaco Michel eFayol Rebecca eTreiman |
author_sort | Sebastien ePacton |
collection | DOAJ |
description | Two experiments investigated whether and how the learning of spellings by French third graders is influenced by two graphotactic patterns: consonants cannot double in word-initial position (Experiment 1) and consonants cannot double after single consonants (Experiment 2). Children silently read meaningful texts that contained three types of novel spellings: no doublet (e.g., mupile, guprane), doublet in a legal position (e.g., muppile, gupprane), and doublet in an illegal position (e.g., mmupile, guprrane). Orthographic learning was assessed with a task of spelling to dictation. In both experiments, children recalled items without doublets better than items with doublets. In Experiment 1, children recalled spellings with a doublet in illegal word-initial position better than spellings with a doublet in legal word-medial position, and almost all misspellings involved the omission of the doublet. The fact that the graphotactic violation in an item like mmupile was in the salient initial position may explain why children often remembered both the presence and the position of the doublet. In Experiment 2, children recalled nonwords with a doublet before a single consonant (legal, e.g., gupprane) better than those with a doublet after a single consonant (illegal, e.g., guprrane). Omission of the doublet was the most frequent error for both types of items. Children also made some transposition errors on items with a doublet after a single consonant, recalling for example gupprane instead of guprrane. These results suggest that, when a doublet is in the hard-to-remember medial position, children sometimes remember that an item contains a doublet but not which letter is doubled. Their knowledge that double consonants can occur before but not after single consonants leads to transposition errors on items like guprrane. These results shed new light on when and how children use general knowledge about the graphotactic patterns of their writing system to reconstruct spellings. |
first_indexed | 2024-12-23T04:41:24Z |
format | Article |
id | doaj.art-e156d34ed6c5487b96507843760f6922 |
institution | Directory Open Access Journal |
issn | 1664-1078 |
language | English |
last_indexed | 2024-12-23T04:41:24Z |
publishDate | 2013-10-01 |
publisher | Frontiers Media S.A. |
record_format | Article |
series | Frontiers in Psychology |
spelling | doaj.art-e156d34ed6c5487b96507843760f69222022-12-21T17:59:47ZengFrontiers Media S.A.Frontiers in Psychology1664-10782013-10-01410.3389/fpsyg.2013.0070161323How does graphotactic knowledge influence children’s learning of new spellings?Sebastien ePacton0Amélie eSobaco1Amélie eSobaco2Michel eFayol3Rebecca eTreiman4Université Paris Descartes - Laboratoire Mémoire et CognitionUniversité Paris Descartes - Laboratoire Mémoire et CognitionUniversité Paris DescartesUniversité Blaise PascalWashington University in St. LouisTwo experiments investigated whether and how the learning of spellings by French third graders is influenced by two graphotactic patterns: consonants cannot double in word-initial position (Experiment 1) and consonants cannot double after single consonants (Experiment 2). Children silently read meaningful texts that contained three types of novel spellings: no doublet (e.g., mupile, guprane), doublet in a legal position (e.g., muppile, gupprane), and doublet in an illegal position (e.g., mmupile, guprrane). Orthographic learning was assessed with a task of spelling to dictation. In both experiments, children recalled items without doublets better than items with doublets. In Experiment 1, children recalled spellings with a doublet in illegal word-initial position better than spellings with a doublet in legal word-medial position, and almost all misspellings involved the omission of the doublet. The fact that the graphotactic violation in an item like mmupile was in the salient initial position may explain why children often remembered both the presence and the position of the doublet. In Experiment 2, children recalled nonwords with a doublet before a single consonant (legal, e.g., gupprane) better than those with a doublet after a single consonant (illegal, e.g., guprrane). Omission of the doublet was the most frequent error for both types of items. Children also made some transposition errors on items with a doublet after a single consonant, recalling for example gupprane instead of guprrane. These results suggest that, when a doublet is in the hard-to-remember medial position, children sometimes remember that an item contains a doublet but not which letter is doubled. Their knowledge that double consonants can occur before but not after single consonants leads to transposition errors on items like guprrane. These results shed new light on when and how children use general knowledge about the graphotactic patterns of their writing system to reconstruct spellings.http://journal.frontiersin.org/Journal/10.3389/fpsyg.2013.00701/fullimplicit learningstatistical learningspellinggraphotacicsorthographic learning |
spellingShingle | Sebastien ePacton Amélie eSobaco Amélie eSobaco Michel eFayol Rebecca eTreiman How does graphotactic knowledge influence children’s learning of new spellings? Frontiers in Psychology implicit learning statistical learning spelling graphotacics orthographic learning |
title | How does graphotactic knowledge influence children’s learning of new spellings? |
title_full | How does graphotactic knowledge influence children’s learning of new spellings? |
title_fullStr | How does graphotactic knowledge influence children’s learning of new spellings? |
title_full_unstemmed | How does graphotactic knowledge influence children’s learning of new spellings? |
title_short | How does graphotactic knowledge influence children’s learning of new spellings? |
title_sort | how does graphotactic knowledge influence children s learning of new spellings |
topic | implicit learning statistical learning spelling graphotacics orthographic learning |
url | http://journal.frontiersin.org/Journal/10.3389/fpsyg.2013.00701/full |
work_keys_str_mv | AT sebastienepacton howdoesgraphotacticknowledgeinfluencechildrenslearningofnewspellings AT amelieesobaco howdoesgraphotacticknowledgeinfluencechildrenslearningofnewspellings AT amelieesobaco howdoesgraphotacticknowledgeinfluencechildrenslearningofnewspellings AT michelefayol howdoesgraphotacticknowledgeinfluencechildrenslearningofnewspellings AT rebeccaetreiman howdoesgraphotacticknowledgeinfluencechildrenslearningofnewspellings |