Nocturnal incubation recess and flushing behavior by duck hens
Abstract Incubating birds must balance the needs of their developing embryos with their own physiological needs, and many birds accomplish this by taking periodic breaks from incubation. Mallard (Anas platyrhynchos) and gadwall (Mareca strepera) hens typically take incubation recesses in the early m...
Main Authors: | , , , , , , |
---|---|
Format: | Article |
Language: | English |
Published: |
Wiley
2021-06-01
|
Series: | Ecology and Evolution |
Subjects: | |
Online Access: | https://doi.org/10.1002/ece3.7561 |
_version_ | 1819045236675969024 |
---|---|
author | Rebecca Croston Sarah H. Peterson C. Alex Hartman Mark P. Herzog Cliff L. Feldheim Michael L. Casazza Joshua T. Ackerman |
author_facet | Rebecca Croston Sarah H. Peterson C. Alex Hartman Mark P. Herzog Cliff L. Feldheim Michael L. Casazza Joshua T. Ackerman |
author_sort | Rebecca Croston |
collection | DOAJ |
description | Abstract Incubating birds must balance the needs of their developing embryos with their own physiological needs, and many birds accomplish this by taking periodic breaks from incubation. Mallard (Anas platyrhynchos) and gadwall (Mareca strepera) hens typically take incubation recesses in the early morning and late afternoon, but recesses can also take place at night. We examined nocturnal incubation recess behavior for mallard and gadwall hens nesting in Suisun Marsh, California, USA, using iButton temperature dataloggers and continuous video monitoring at nests. Fourteen percent of all detected incubation recesses (N = 13,708) were nocturnal and took place on 20% of nest‐days (N = 8,668). Video monitoring showed that hens covered their eggs with down feathers when they initiated a nocturnal recess themselves as they would a diurnal recess, but they left the eggs uncovered in 94% of the nocturnal recesses in which predators appeared at nests. Thus, determining whether or not eggs were left uncovered during a recess can provide strong indication whether the recess was initiated by the hen (eggs covered) or a predator (eggs uncovered). Because nest temperature decreased more rapidly when eggs were left uncovered versus covered, we were able to characterize eggs during nocturnal incubation recesses as covered or uncovered using nest temperature data. Overall, we predicted that 75% of nocturnal recesses were hen‐initiated recesses (eggs covered) whereas 25% of nocturnal recesses were predator‐initiated recesses (eggs uncovered). Of the predator‐initiated nocturnal recesses, 56% were accompanied by evidence of depredation at the nest during the subsequent nest monitoring visit. Hen‐initiated nocturnal recesses began later in the night (closer to morning) and were shorter than predator‐initiated nocturnal recesses. Our results indicate that nocturnal incubation recesses occur regularly (14% of all recesses) and, similar to diurnal recesses, most nocturnal recesses (75%) are initiated by the hen rather than an approaching predator. |
first_indexed | 2024-12-21T10:25:22Z |
format | Article |
id | doaj.art-e8bc70f5edb34642ac37d6a9472285c2 |
institution | Directory Open Access Journal |
issn | 2045-7758 |
language | English |
last_indexed | 2024-12-21T10:25:22Z |
publishDate | 2021-06-01 |
publisher | Wiley |
record_format | Article |
series | Ecology and Evolution |
spelling | doaj.art-e8bc70f5edb34642ac37d6a9472285c22022-12-21T19:07:20ZengWileyEcology and Evolution2045-77582021-06-0111127292730110.1002/ece3.7561Nocturnal incubation recess and flushing behavior by duck hensRebecca Croston0Sarah H. Peterson1C. Alex Hartman2Mark P. Herzog3Cliff L. Feldheim4Michael L. Casazza5Joshua T. Ackerman6U.S. Geological Survey Western Ecological Research Center Dixon Field Station Dixon CA USAU.S. Geological Survey Western Ecological Research Center Dixon Field Station Dixon CA USAU.S. Geological Survey Western Ecological Research Center Dixon Field Station Dixon CA USAU.S. Geological Survey Western Ecological Research Center Dixon Field Station Dixon CA USASuisun Marsh Program California Department of Water Resources Sacramento CA USAU.S. Geological Survey Western Ecological Research Center Dixon Field Station Dixon CA USAU.S. Geological Survey Western Ecological Research Center Dixon Field Station Dixon CA USAAbstract Incubating birds must balance the needs of their developing embryos with their own physiological needs, and many birds accomplish this by taking periodic breaks from incubation. Mallard (Anas platyrhynchos) and gadwall (Mareca strepera) hens typically take incubation recesses in the early morning and late afternoon, but recesses can also take place at night. We examined nocturnal incubation recess behavior for mallard and gadwall hens nesting in Suisun Marsh, California, USA, using iButton temperature dataloggers and continuous video monitoring at nests. Fourteen percent of all detected incubation recesses (N = 13,708) were nocturnal and took place on 20% of nest‐days (N = 8,668). Video monitoring showed that hens covered their eggs with down feathers when they initiated a nocturnal recess themselves as they would a diurnal recess, but they left the eggs uncovered in 94% of the nocturnal recesses in which predators appeared at nests. Thus, determining whether or not eggs were left uncovered during a recess can provide strong indication whether the recess was initiated by the hen (eggs covered) or a predator (eggs uncovered). Because nest temperature decreased more rapidly when eggs were left uncovered versus covered, we were able to characterize eggs during nocturnal incubation recesses as covered or uncovered using nest temperature data. Overall, we predicted that 75% of nocturnal recesses were hen‐initiated recesses (eggs covered) whereas 25% of nocturnal recesses were predator‐initiated recesses (eggs uncovered). Of the predator‐initiated nocturnal recesses, 56% were accompanied by evidence of depredation at the nest during the subsequent nest monitoring visit. Hen‐initiated nocturnal recesses began later in the night (closer to morning) and were shorter than predator‐initiated nocturnal recesses. Our results indicate that nocturnal incubation recesses occur regularly (14% of all recesses) and, similar to diurnal recesses, most nocturnal recesses (75%) are initiated by the hen rather than an approaching predator.https://doi.org/10.1002/ece3.7561dabbling duckgadwalliButtonincubation recessmallardnest depredation |
spellingShingle | Rebecca Croston Sarah H. Peterson C. Alex Hartman Mark P. Herzog Cliff L. Feldheim Michael L. Casazza Joshua T. Ackerman Nocturnal incubation recess and flushing behavior by duck hens Ecology and Evolution dabbling duck gadwall iButton incubation recess mallard nest depredation |
title | Nocturnal incubation recess and flushing behavior by duck hens |
title_full | Nocturnal incubation recess and flushing behavior by duck hens |
title_fullStr | Nocturnal incubation recess and flushing behavior by duck hens |
title_full_unstemmed | Nocturnal incubation recess and flushing behavior by duck hens |
title_short | Nocturnal incubation recess and flushing behavior by duck hens |
title_sort | nocturnal incubation recess and flushing behavior by duck hens |
topic | dabbling duck gadwall iButton incubation recess mallard nest depredation |
url | https://doi.org/10.1002/ece3.7561 |
work_keys_str_mv | AT rebeccacroston nocturnalincubationrecessandflushingbehaviorbyduckhens AT sarahhpeterson nocturnalincubationrecessandflushingbehaviorbyduckhens AT calexhartman nocturnalincubationrecessandflushingbehaviorbyduckhens AT markpherzog nocturnalincubationrecessandflushingbehaviorbyduckhens AT clifflfeldheim nocturnalincubationrecessandflushingbehaviorbyduckhens AT michaellcasazza nocturnalincubationrecessandflushingbehaviorbyduckhens AT joshuatackerman nocturnalincubationrecessandflushingbehaviorbyduckhens |