The Contributions of Positive and Negative Affect to Emotional Well-Being

In this paper, the definitions of subjective well-being have been reviewed with a focus on its emotional core, which we consider to be the ratio of positive to negative affect over time. The reviewed evidence showed that negative emotions tend to be of longer duration than positive and that the NA s...

Full description

Bibliographic Details
Main Author: Randy Larsen
Format: Article
Language:English
Published: University of Rijeka 2009-12-01
Series:Psychological Topics
Subjects:
Online Access:http://hrcak.srce.hr/file/74336
_version_ 1828518766199177216
author Randy Larsen
author_facet Randy Larsen
author_sort Randy Larsen
collection DOAJ
description In this paper, the definitions of subjective well-being have been reviewed with a focus on its emotional core, which we consider to be the ratio of positive to negative affect over time. The reviewed evidence showed that negative emotions tend to be of longer duration than positive and that the NA system produces stronger emotional responses than the PA system. Also, a variety of experimental results show that negative stimuli make unique demands on cognitive resources (particularly perception and attention) compared to positive stimuli. The evidence that the negative affect system produces stronger affective output, per unit input, than the positive affect system, is a phenomenon known as negativity bias. I also went so far as to argue that negativity exceeds positivity by a factor of pi (3.14) and that efforts to speed adaptation to negative events may be more important to overall SWB then efforts to prolong responses to positive events (Larsen and Prizmic, 2008). The fact that negativity is stronger than positivity, combined with the notion of differential adaptation (people adapt faster to good events than to bad events), creates the conditions that drive the hedonic treadmill. However, most people are, to some degree, able to overcome the psychological forces of the hedonic treadmill and maintain at least a modicum of emotional well-being (Biswas-Diener, Vitterso, & Diener, 2005). It is likely that the ability called "emotional intelligence" refers in large part to the capacity to manage negative affect following unpleasant or stressful events (Larsen & Learner, 2006). Moreover, such an ability is likely to be made up of particular behaviors and strategies that each contributes specifically to the management of negative emotions (Larsen & Prizmic, 2004).
first_indexed 2024-12-11T19:00:29Z
format Article
id doaj.art-e91c9b4ce7ef49798e567ee6f03c0bd7
institution Directory Open Access Journal
issn 1332-0742
language English
last_indexed 2024-12-11T19:00:29Z
publishDate 2009-12-01
publisher University of Rijeka
record_format Article
series Psychological Topics
spelling doaj.art-e91c9b4ce7ef49798e567ee6f03c0bd72022-12-22T00:54:02ZengUniversity of RijekaPsychological Topics1332-07422009-12-01182247266The Contributions of Positive and Negative Affect to Emotional Well-BeingRandy LarsenIn this paper, the definitions of subjective well-being have been reviewed with a focus on its emotional core, which we consider to be the ratio of positive to negative affect over time. The reviewed evidence showed that negative emotions tend to be of longer duration than positive and that the NA system produces stronger emotional responses than the PA system. Also, a variety of experimental results show that negative stimuli make unique demands on cognitive resources (particularly perception and attention) compared to positive stimuli. The evidence that the negative affect system produces stronger affective output, per unit input, than the positive affect system, is a phenomenon known as negativity bias. I also went so far as to argue that negativity exceeds positivity by a factor of pi (3.14) and that efforts to speed adaptation to negative events may be more important to overall SWB then efforts to prolong responses to positive events (Larsen and Prizmic, 2008). The fact that negativity is stronger than positivity, combined with the notion of differential adaptation (people adapt faster to good events than to bad events), creates the conditions that drive the hedonic treadmill. However, most people are, to some degree, able to overcome the psychological forces of the hedonic treadmill and maintain at least a modicum of emotional well-being (Biswas-Diener, Vitterso, & Diener, 2005). It is likely that the ability called "emotional intelligence" refers in large part to the capacity to manage negative affect following unpleasant or stressful events (Larsen & Learner, 2006). Moreover, such an ability is likely to be made up of particular behaviors and strategies that each contributes specifically to the management of negative emotions (Larsen & Prizmic, 2004).http://hrcak.srce.hr/file/74336emotinal well-beingpositive affectnegative afect
spellingShingle Randy Larsen
The Contributions of Positive and Negative Affect to Emotional Well-Being
Psychological Topics
emotinal well-being
positive affect
negative afect
title The Contributions of Positive and Negative Affect to Emotional Well-Being
title_full The Contributions of Positive and Negative Affect to Emotional Well-Being
title_fullStr The Contributions of Positive and Negative Affect to Emotional Well-Being
title_full_unstemmed The Contributions of Positive and Negative Affect to Emotional Well-Being
title_short The Contributions of Positive and Negative Affect to Emotional Well-Being
title_sort contributions of positive and negative affect to emotional well being
topic emotinal well-being
positive affect
negative afect
url http://hrcak.srce.hr/file/74336
work_keys_str_mv AT randylarsen thecontributionsofpositiveandnegativeaffecttoemotionalwellbeing
AT randylarsen contributionsofpositiveandnegativeaffecttoemotionalwellbeing