10 years of mindlines: a systematic review and commentary

<p><strong>Background: </strong> In 2004, Gabbay and le May showed that clinicians generally base their decisions on mindlines—internalised and collectively reinforced tacit guidelines—rather than consulting written clinical guidelines. We considered how the concept of mindlines ha...

Full description

Bibliographic Details
Main Authors: Wieringa, S, Greenhalgh, T
Format: Journal article
Published: BioMed Central 2015
_version_ 1797052319404130304
author Wieringa, S
Greenhalgh, T
author_facet Wieringa, S
Greenhalgh, T
author_sort Wieringa, S
collection OXFORD
description <p><strong>Background: </strong> In 2004, Gabbay and le May showed that clinicians generally base their decisions on mindlines—internalised and collectively reinforced tacit guidelines—rather than consulting written clinical guidelines. We considered how the concept of mindlines has been taken forward since.</p> <p><strong>Methods: </strong> We searched databases from 2004 to 2014 for the term ‘mindline(s)’ and tracked all sources citing Gabbay and le May’s 2004 article. We read and re-read papers to gain familiarity and developed an interpretive analysis and taxonomy by drawing on the principles of meta-narrative systematic review.</p> <p><strong>Results: </strong> In our synthesis of 340 papers, distinguished between authors who used mindlines purely in name (‘nominal’ view) sometimes dismissing them as a harmful phenomenon, and authors who appeared to have understood the term’s philosophical foundations. The latter took an ‘in-practice’ view (studying how mindlines emerge and spread in real-world settings), a ‘theoretical and philosophical’ view (extending theory) or a ‘solution focused’ view (exploring how to promote and support mindline development). We found that it is not just clinicians who develop mindlines: so do patients, in face-to-face and (potentially) online communities.</p> <p>Theoretical publications on mindlines have continued to challenge the rationalist assumptions of evidence-based medicine (EBM). Conventional EBM assumes a single, knowable reality and seeks to strip away context to generate universal predictive rules. In contrast, mindlines are predicated on a more fluid, embodied and intersubjective view of knowledge; they accommodate context and acknowledge multiple realities. When considering how knowledge spreads, the concept of mindlines requires us to go beyond the constraining notions of ‘dissemination’ and ‘translation’ to study tacit knowledge and the interactive human processes by which such knowledge is created, enacted and shared. Solution-focused publications described mindline-promoting initiatives such as relationship-building, collaborative learning and thought leadership.</p> <p><strong>Conclusions: </strong> The concept of mindlines challenges the naïve rationalist view of knowledge implicit in some EBM publications, but the term appears to have been misunderstood (and prematurely dismissed) by some authors. By further studying mindlines empirically and theoretically, there is potential to expand EBM’s conceptual toolkit to produce richer forms of ‘evidence-based’ knowledge. We outline a suggested research agenda for achieving this goal.</p>
first_indexed 2024-03-06T18:30:27Z
format Journal article
id oxford-uuid:0973d019-a043-4192-bc95-a062ddac6b44
institution University of Oxford
last_indexed 2024-03-06T18:30:27Z
publishDate 2015
publisher BioMed Central
record_format dspace
spelling oxford-uuid:0973d019-a043-4192-bc95-a062ddac6b442022-03-26T09:18:33Z10 years of mindlines: a systematic review and commentaryJournal articlehttp://purl.org/coar/resource_type/c_dcae04bcuuid:0973d019-a043-4192-bc95-a062ddac6b44Symplectic Elements at OxfordBioMed Central2015Wieringa, SGreenhalgh, T<p><strong>Background: </strong> In 2004, Gabbay and le May showed that clinicians generally base their decisions on mindlines—internalised and collectively reinforced tacit guidelines—rather than consulting written clinical guidelines. We considered how the concept of mindlines has been taken forward since.</p> <p><strong>Methods: </strong> We searched databases from 2004 to 2014 for the term ‘mindline(s)’ and tracked all sources citing Gabbay and le May’s 2004 article. We read and re-read papers to gain familiarity and developed an interpretive analysis and taxonomy by drawing on the principles of meta-narrative systematic review.</p> <p><strong>Results: </strong> In our synthesis of 340 papers, distinguished between authors who used mindlines purely in name (‘nominal’ view) sometimes dismissing them as a harmful phenomenon, and authors who appeared to have understood the term’s philosophical foundations. The latter took an ‘in-practice’ view (studying how mindlines emerge and spread in real-world settings), a ‘theoretical and philosophical’ view (extending theory) or a ‘solution focused’ view (exploring how to promote and support mindline development). We found that it is not just clinicians who develop mindlines: so do patients, in face-to-face and (potentially) online communities.</p> <p>Theoretical publications on mindlines have continued to challenge the rationalist assumptions of evidence-based medicine (EBM). Conventional EBM assumes a single, knowable reality and seeks to strip away context to generate universal predictive rules. In contrast, mindlines are predicated on a more fluid, embodied and intersubjective view of knowledge; they accommodate context and acknowledge multiple realities. When considering how knowledge spreads, the concept of mindlines requires us to go beyond the constraining notions of ‘dissemination’ and ‘translation’ to study tacit knowledge and the interactive human processes by which such knowledge is created, enacted and shared. Solution-focused publications described mindline-promoting initiatives such as relationship-building, collaborative learning and thought leadership.</p> <p><strong>Conclusions: </strong> The concept of mindlines challenges the naïve rationalist view of knowledge implicit in some EBM publications, but the term appears to have been misunderstood (and prematurely dismissed) by some authors. By further studying mindlines empirically and theoretically, there is potential to expand EBM’s conceptual toolkit to produce richer forms of ‘evidence-based’ knowledge. We outline a suggested research agenda for achieving this goal.</p>
spellingShingle Wieringa, S
Greenhalgh, T
10 years of mindlines: a systematic review and commentary
title 10 years of mindlines: a systematic review and commentary
title_full 10 years of mindlines: a systematic review and commentary
title_fullStr 10 years of mindlines: a systematic review and commentary
title_full_unstemmed 10 years of mindlines: a systematic review and commentary
title_short 10 years of mindlines: a systematic review and commentary
title_sort 10 years of mindlines a systematic review and commentary
work_keys_str_mv AT wieringas 10yearsofmindlinesasystematicreviewandcommentary
AT greenhalght 10yearsofmindlinesasystematicreviewandcommentary