Three timescales in prism adaptation

<p style="text-align:justify;"> It has been proposed that motor adaptation depends on at least two learning systems, one that learns fast but with poor retention and another that learns slowly but with better retention (Smith MA, Ghazizadeh A, Shadmehr R. PLoS Biol 4: e179, 2006). T...

Ամբողջական նկարագրություն

Մատենագիտական մանրամասներ
Հիմնական հեղինակներ: Inoue, M, Uchimura, M, Karibe, A, O'Shea, J, Rossetti, Y, Kitazawa, S
Ձևաչափ: Journal article
Լեզու:English
Հրապարակվել է: American Physiological Society 2015
_version_ 1826279660400410624
author Inoue, M
Uchimura, M
Karibe, A
O'Shea, J
Rossetti, Y
Kitazawa, S
author_facet Inoue, M
Uchimura, M
Karibe, A
O'Shea, J
Rossetti, Y
Kitazawa, S
author_sort Inoue, M
collection OXFORD
description <p style="text-align:justify;"> It has been proposed that motor adaptation depends on at least two learning systems, one that learns fast but with poor retention and another that learns slowly but with better retention (Smith MA, Ghazizadeh A, Shadmehr R. PLoS Biol 4: e179, 2006). This two-state model has been shown to account for a range of behavior in the force field adaptation task. In the present study, we examined whether such a two-state model could also account for behavior arising from adaptation to a prismatic displacement of the visual field. We first confirmed that an “adaptation rebound,” a critical prediction of the two-state model, occurred when visual feedback was deprived after an adaptation-extinction episode. We then examined the speed of decay of the prism aftereffect (without any visual feedback) after repetitions of 30, 150, and 500 trials of prism exposure. The speed of decay decreased with the number of exposure trials, a phenomenon that was best explained by assuming an “ultraslow” system, in addition to the fast and slow systems. Finally, we compared retention of aftereffects 24 h after 150 or 500 trials of exposure: retention was significantly greater after 500 than 150 trials. This difference in retention could not be explained by the two-state model but was well explained by the three-state model as arising from the difference in the amount of adaptation of the “ultraslow process.” These results suggest that there are not only fast and slow systems but also an ultraslow learning system in prism adaptation that is activated by prolonged prism exposure of 150–500 trials. </p>
first_indexed 2024-03-07T00:02:05Z
format Journal article
id oxford-uuid:764c0b82-c7d6-4fe6-88c2-beef8aa1c98a
institution University of Oxford
language English
last_indexed 2024-03-07T00:02:05Z
publishDate 2015
publisher American Physiological Society
record_format dspace
spelling oxford-uuid:764c0b82-c7d6-4fe6-88c2-beef8aa1c98a2022-03-26T20:14:55ZThree timescales in prism adaptationJournal articlehttp://purl.org/coar/resource_type/c_dcae04bcuuid:764c0b82-c7d6-4fe6-88c2-beef8aa1c98aEnglishSymplectic Elements at OxfordAmerican Physiological Society2015Inoue, MUchimura, MKaribe, AO'Shea, JRossetti, YKitazawa, S <p style="text-align:justify;"> It has been proposed that motor adaptation depends on at least two learning systems, one that learns fast but with poor retention and another that learns slowly but with better retention (Smith MA, Ghazizadeh A, Shadmehr R. PLoS Biol 4: e179, 2006). This two-state model has been shown to account for a range of behavior in the force field adaptation task. In the present study, we examined whether such a two-state model could also account for behavior arising from adaptation to a prismatic displacement of the visual field. We first confirmed that an “adaptation rebound,” a critical prediction of the two-state model, occurred when visual feedback was deprived after an adaptation-extinction episode. We then examined the speed of decay of the prism aftereffect (without any visual feedback) after repetitions of 30, 150, and 500 trials of prism exposure. The speed of decay decreased with the number of exposure trials, a phenomenon that was best explained by assuming an “ultraslow” system, in addition to the fast and slow systems. Finally, we compared retention of aftereffects 24 h after 150 or 500 trials of exposure: retention was significantly greater after 500 than 150 trials. This difference in retention could not be explained by the two-state model but was well explained by the three-state model as arising from the difference in the amount of adaptation of the “ultraslow process.” These results suggest that there are not only fast and slow systems but also an ultraslow learning system in prism adaptation that is activated by prolonged prism exposure of 150–500 trials. </p>
spellingShingle Inoue, M
Uchimura, M
Karibe, A
O'Shea, J
Rossetti, Y
Kitazawa, S
Three timescales in prism adaptation
title Three timescales in prism adaptation
title_full Three timescales in prism adaptation
title_fullStr Three timescales in prism adaptation
title_full_unstemmed Three timescales in prism adaptation
title_short Three timescales in prism adaptation
title_sort three timescales in prism adaptation
work_keys_str_mv AT inouem threetimescalesinprismadaptation
AT uchimuram threetimescalesinprismadaptation
AT karibea threetimescalesinprismadaptation
AT osheaj threetimescalesinprismadaptation
AT rossettiy threetimescalesinprismadaptation
AT kitazawas threetimescalesinprismadaptation