Linking language to sensory experience: onomatopoeia in early language development

A key question in developmental research concerns how children learn associations between words and meanings in their early language development. Given a vast array of possible referents, how does the child know what a word refers to? We contend that onomatopoeia (e.g. knock, meow), where a word...

Full description

Bibliographic Details
Main Authors: Motamedi, Y, Murgiano, M, Perniss, P, Wonnacott, E, Marshall, C, Goldin-Meadow, S, Vigliocco, G
Format: Journal article
Language:English
Published: Wiley 2020
_version_ 1797085882935672832
author Motamedi, Y
Murgiano, M
Perniss, P
Wonnacott, E
Marshall, C
Goldin-Meadow, S
Vigliocco, G
author_facet Motamedi, Y
Murgiano, M
Perniss, P
Wonnacott, E
Marshall, C
Goldin-Meadow, S
Vigliocco, G
author_sort Motamedi, Y
collection OXFORD
description A key question in developmental research concerns how children learn associations between words and meanings in their early language development. Given a vast array of possible referents, how does the child know what a word refers to? We contend that onomatopoeia (e.g. knock, meow), where a word's sound evokes the sound properties associated with its meaning, are particularly useful in children's early vocabulary development, offering a link between word and sensory experience not present in arbitrary forms. We suggest that, because onomatopoeia evoke imagery of the referent, children can draw from sensory experience to easily link onomatopoeic words to meaning, both when the referent is present as well as when it is absent. We use two sources of data: naturalistic observations of English‐speaking caregiver–child interactions from 14 up to 54 months, to establish whether these words are present early in caregivers’ speech to children, and experimental data to test whether English‐speaking children can learn from onomatopoeia when it is present. Our results demonstrate that onomatopoeia: (a) are most prevalent in early child‐directed language and in children's early productions, (b) are learnt more easily by children compared with non‐iconic forms and (c) are used by caregivers in contexts where they can support communication and facilitate word learning.
first_indexed 2024-03-07T02:14:13Z
format Journal article
id oxford-uuid:a1aa65ab-ee43-4c81-8d20-7f5e2b36d3ad
institution University of Oxford
language English
last_indexed 2024-03-07T02:14:13Z
publishDate 2020
publisher Wiley
record_format dspace
spelling oxford-uuid:a1aa65ab-ee43-4c81-8d20-7f5e2b36d3ad2022-03-27T02:14:50ZLinking language to sensory experience: onomatopoeia in early language developmentJournal articlehttp://purl.org/coar/resource_type/c_dcae04bcuuid:a1aa65ab-ee43-4c81-8d20-7f5e2b36d3adEnglishSymplectic ElementsWiley2020Motamedi, YMurgiano, MPerniss, PWonnacott, EMarshall, CGoldin-Meadow, SVigliocco, GA key question in developmental research concerns how children learn associations between words and meanings in their early language development. Given a vast array of possible referents, how does the child know what a word refers to? We contend that onomatopoeia (e.g. knock, meow), where a word's sound evokes the sound properties associated with its meaning, are particularly useful in children's early vocabulary development, offering a link between word and sensory experience not present in arbitrary forms. We suggest that, because onomatopoeia evoke imagery of the referent, children can draw from sensory experience to easily link onomatopoeic words to meaning, both when the referent is present as well as when it is absent. We use two sources of data: naturalistic observations of English‐speaking caregiver–child interactions from 14 up to 54 months, to establish whether these words are present early in caregivers’ speech to children, and experimental data to test whether English‐speaking children can learn from onomatopoeia when it is present. Our results demonstrate that onomatopoeia: (a) are most prevalent in early child‐directed language and in children's early productions, (b) are learnt more easily by children compared with non‐iconic forms and (c) are used by caregivers in contexts where they can support communication and facilitate word learning.
spellingShingle Motamedi, Y
Murgiano, M
Perniss, P
Wonnacott, E
Marshall, C
Goldin-Meadow, S
Vigliocco, G
Linking language to sensory experience: onomatopoeia in early language development
title Linking language to sensory experience: onomatopoeia in early language development
title_full Linking language to sensory experience: onomatopoeia in early language development
title_fullStr Linking language to sensory experience: onomatopoeia in early language development
title_full_unstemmed Linking language to sensory experience: onomatopoeia in early language development
title_short Linking language to sensory experience: onomatopoeia in early language development
title_sort linking language to sensory experience onomatopoeia in early language development
work_keys_str_mv AT motamediy linkinglanguagetosensoryexperienceonomatopoeiainearlylanguagedevelopment
AT murgianom linkinglanguagetosensoryexperienceonomatopoeiainearlylanguagedevelopment
AT pernissp linkinglanguagetosensoryexperienceonomatopoeiainearlylanguagedevelopment
AT wonnacotte linkinglanguagetosensoryexperienceonomatopoeiainearlylanguagedevelopment
AT marshallc linkinglanguagetosensoryexperienceonomatopoeiainearlylanguagedevelopment
AT goldinmeadows linkinglanguagetosensoryexperienceonomatopoeiainearlylanguagedevelopment
AT viglioccog linkinglanguagetosensoryexperienceonomatopoeiainearlylanguagedevelopment