Impact of Early and Concurrent Stunting on Cognition

<p>Undernutrition is associated with poor cognitive development, late entry into school, decreased years of schooling, reduced productivity, and smaller adult stature. We use longitudinal data from 1674 Peruvian children participating in the Young Lives study to assess the relative impact of e...

Full description

Bibliographic Details
Main Authors: Crookston, B, Dearden, K, Alder, S, Porucznik, C, Stanford, J, Merrill, R, Dickerson, T, Penny, M
Format: Journal article
Language:English
Published: John Wiley & Sons, Inc 2011
Subjects:
_version_ 1826289802273619968
author Crookston, B
Dearden, K
Alder, S
Porucznik, C
Stanford, J
Merrill, R
Dickerson, T
Penny, M
author_facet Crookston, B
Dearden, K
Alder, S
Porucznik, C
Stanford, J
Merrill, R
Dickerson, T
Penny, M
author_sort Crookston, B
collection OXFORD
description <p>Undernutrition is associated with poor cognitive development, late entry into school, decreased years of schooling, reduced productivity, and smaller adult stature. We use longitudinal data from 1674 Peruvian children participating in the Young Lives study to assess the relative impact of early stunting (stunted at 6-18 months of age) and concurrent stunting (stunted at 4.5-6 years of age) on cognitive ability. Anthropometric data were longitudinally collected for children at 6-18 months of age and 4.5-6 years of age at which time verbal and quantitative ability were also assessed. We estimate that an increase in concurrent height-for-age z-scores (HAZ) by one standard deviation was associated with an increase in a child’s score on the Peabody Picture Vocabulary Test (PPVT) by 2.35 points (CI: 1.55-3.15) and a 0.16 point increase on the Cognitive Development Assessment (CDA) (CI: 0.05-0.27). Further, we report that the estimate for concurrent HAZ and PPVT is significantly higher than the estimate for early stunting and PPVT. We found no significant difference between early and concurrent estimates for HAZ and CDA. Children from older mothers, children whose mothers had higher education levels, children living in urban areas, children who attended preschool, children with fewer siblings, and children from wealthier backgrounds scored higher on both assessments. Cognitive skills of children entering school were associated with early stunting but the strongest association was found with concurrent stunting suggesting that interventions preventing linear growth faltering should not only focus on the under twos but include children up to five years of age.</p>
first_indexed 2024-03-07T02:34:27Z
format Journal article
id oxford-uuid:a851ba05-dcc7-4764-8637-8d74c1ebcbf1
institution University of Oxford
language English
last_indexed 2024-03-07T02:34:27Z
publishDate 2011
publisher John Wiley & Sons, Inc
record_format dspace
spelling oxford-uuid:a851ba05-dcc7-4764-8637-8d74c1ebcbf12022-03-27T03:00:44ZImpact of Early and Concurrent Stunting on CognitionJournal articlehttp://purl.org/coar/resource_type/c_dcae04bcuuid:a851ba05-dcc7-4764-8637-8d74c1ebcbf1Cognitive developmentEnglishOxford University Research Archive - ValetJohn Wiley & Sons, Inc2011Crookston, BDearden, KAlder, SPorucznik, CStanford, JMerrill, RDickerson, TPenny, M<p>Undernutrition is associated with poor cognitive development, late entry into school, decreased years of schooling, reduced productivity, and smaller adult stature. We use longitudinal data from 1674 Peruvian children participating in the Young Lives study to assess the relative impact of early stunting (stunted at 6-18 months of age) and concurrent stunting (stunted at 4.5-6 years of age) on cognitive ability. Anthropometric data were longitudinally collected for children at 6-18 months of age and 4.5-6 years of age at which time verbal and quantitative ability were also assessed. We estimate that an increase in concurrent height-for-age z-scores (HAZ) by one standard deviation was associated with an increase in a child’s score on the Peabody Picture Vocabulary Test (PPVT) by 2.35 points (CI: 1.55-3.15) and a 0.16 point increase on the Cognitive Development Assessment (CDA) (CI: 0.05-0.27). Further, we report that the estimate for concurrent HAZ and PPVT is significantly higher than the estimate for early stunting and PPVT. We found no significant difference between early and concurrent estimates for HAZ and CDA. Children from older mothers, children whose mothers had higher education levels, children living in urban areas, children who attended preschool, children with fewer siblings, and children from wealthier backgrounds scored higher on both assessments. Cognitive skills of children entering school were associated with early stunting but the strongest association was found with concurrent stunting suggesting that interventions preventing linear growth faltering should not only focus on the under twos but include children up to five years of age.</p>
spellingShingle Cognitive development
Crookston, B
Dearden, K
Alder, S
Porucznik, C
Stanford, J
Merrill, R
Dickerson, T
Penny, M
Impact of Early and Concurrent Stunting on Cognition
title Impact of Early and Concurrent Stunting on Cognition
title_full Impact of Early and Concurrent Stunting on Cognition
title_fullStr Impact of Early and Concurrent Stunting on Cognition
title_full_unstemmed Impact of Early and Concurrent Stunting on Cognition
title_short Impact of Early and Concurrent Stunting on Cognition
title_sort impact of early and concurrent stunting on cognition
topic Cognitive development
work_keys_str_mv AT crookstonb impactofearlyandconcurrentstuntingoncognition
AT deardenk impactofearlyandconcurrentstuntingoncognition
AT alders impactofearlyandconcurrentstuntingoncognition
AT porucznikc impactofearlyandconcurrentstuntingoncognition
AT stanfordj impactofearlyandconcurrentstuntingoncognition
AT merrillr impactofearlyandconcurrentstuntingoncognition
AT dickersont impactofearlyandconcurrentstuntingoncognition
AT pennym impactofearlyandconcurrentstuntingoncognition