The reliability of eyetracking to assess attentional bias to threatening words in healthy individuals
Eyetracking is commonly used to investigate attentional bias. Although some studies have investigated the internal consistency of eyetracking, data are scarce on the test–retest reliability and agreement of eyetracking to investigate attentional bias. This study reports the test–retest reliability,...
Những tác giả chính: | , , , , , , , |
---|---|
Định dạng: | Journal article |
Ngôn ngữ: | English |
Được phát hành: |
Springer
2017
|
_version_ | 1826294542532345856 |
---|---|
author | Skinner, I Hübscher, M Moseley, G Lee, H Wand, B Traeger, A Gustin, S McAuley, J |
author_facet | Skinner, I Hübscher, M Moseley, G Lee, H Wand, B Traeger, A Gustin, S McAuley, J |
author_sort | Skinner, I |
collection | OXFORD |
description | Eyetracking is commonly used to investigate attentional bias. Although some studies have investigated the internal consistency of eyetracking, data are scarce on the test–retest reliability and agreement of eyetracking to investigate attentional bias. This study reports the test–retest reliability, measurement error, and internal consistency of 12 commonly used outcome measures thought to reflect the different components of attentional bias: overall attention, early attention, and late attention. Healthy participants completed a preferential-looking eyetracking task that involved the presentation of threatening (sensory words, general threat words, and affective words) and nonthreatening words. We used intraclass correlation coefficients (ICCs) to measure test–retest reliability (ICC > .70 indicates adequate reliability). The ICCs(2, 1) ranged from –.31 to .71. Reliability varied according to the outcome measure and threat word category. Sensory words had a lower mean ICC (.08) than either affective words (.32) or general threat words (.29). A longer exposure time was associated with higher test–retest reliability. All of the outcome measures, except second-run dwell time, demonstrated low measurement error (≺6%). Most of the outcome measures reported high internal consistency (α ≻ .93). Recommendations are discussed for improving the reliability of eyetracking tasks in future research. |
first_indexed | 2024-03-07T03:47:16Z |
format | Journal article |
id | oxford-uuid:bfe8f5fb-d760-47b7-af7f-939fac66c9a9 |
institution | University of Oxford |
language | English |
last_indexed | 2024-03-07T03:47:16Z |
publishDate | 2017 |
publisher | Springer |
record_format | dspace |
spelling | oxford-uuid:bfe8f5fb-d760-47b7-af7f-939fac66c9a92022-03-27T05:51:01ZThe reliability of eyetracking to assess attentional bias to threatening words in healthy individualsJournal articlehttp://purl.org/coar/resource_type/c_dcae04bcuuid:bfe8f5fb-d760-47b7-af7f-939fac66c9a9EnglishSymplectic Elements at OxfordSpringer2017Skinner, IHübscher, MMoseley, GLee, HWand, BTraeger, AGustin, SMcAuley, JEyetracking is commonly used to investigate attentional bias. Although some studies have investigated the internal consistency of eyetracking, data are scarce on the test–retest reliability and agreement of eyetracking to investigate attentional bias. This study reports the test–retest reliability, measurement error, and internal consistency of 12 commonly used outcome measures thought to reflect the different components of attentional bias: overall attention, early attention, and late attention. Healthy participants completed a preferential-looking eyetracking task that involved the presentation of threatening (sensory words, general threat words, and affective words) and nonthreatening words. We used intraclass correlation coefficients (ICCs) to measure test–retest reliability (ICC > .70 indicates adequate reliability). The ICCs(2, 1) ranged from –.31 to .71. Reliability varied according to the outcome measure and threat word category. Sensory words had a lower mean ICC (.08) than either affective words (.32) or general threat words (.29). A longer exposure time was associated with higher test–retest reliability. All of the outcome measures, except second-run dwell time, demonstrated low measurement error (≺6%). Most of the outcome measures reported high internal consistency (α ≻ .93). Recommendations are discussed for improving the reliability of eyetracking tasks in future research. |
spellingShingle | Skinner, I Hübscher, M Moseley, G Lee, H Wand, B Traeger, A Gustin, S McAuley, J The reliability of eyetracking to assess attentional bias to threatening words in healthy individuals |
title | The reliability of eyetracking to assess attentional bias to threatening words in healthy individuals |
title_full | The reliability of eyetracking to assess attentional bias to threatening words in healthy individuals |
title_fullStr | The reliability of eyetracking to assess attentional bias to threatening words in healthy individuals |
title_full_unstemmed | The reliability of eyetracking to assess attentional bias to threatening words in healthy individuals |
title_short | The reliability of eyetracking to assess attentional bias to threatening words in healthy individuals |
title_sort | reliability of eyetracking to assess attentional bias to threatening words in healthy individuals |
work_keys_str_mv | AT skinneri thereliabilityofeyetrackingtoassessattentionalbiastothreateningwordsinhealthyindividuals AT hubscherm thereliabilityofeyetrackingtoassessattentionalbiastothreateningwordsinhealthyindividuals AT moseleyg thereliabilityofeyetrackingtoassessattentionalbiastothreateningwordsinhealthyindividuals AT leeh thereliabilityofeyetrackingtoassessattentionalbiastothreateningwordsinhealthyindividuals AT wandb thereliabilityofeyetrackingtoassessattentionalbiastothreateningwordsinhealthyindividuals AT traegera thereliabilityofeyetrackingtoassessattentionalbiastothreateningwordsinhealthyindividuals AT gustins thereliabilityofeyetrackingtoassessattentionalbiastothreateningwordsinhealthyindividuals AT mcauleyj thereliabilityofeyetrackingtoassessattentionalbiastothreateningwordsinhealthyindividuals AT skinneri reliabilityofeyetrackingtoassessattentionalbiastothreateningwordsinhealthyindividuals AT hubscherm reliabilityofeyetrackingtoassessattentionalbiastothreateningwordsinhealthyindividuals AT moseleyg reliabilityofeyetrackingtoassessattentionalbiastothreateningwordsinhealthyindividuals AT leeh reliabilityofeyetrackingtoassessattentionalbiastothreateningwordsinhealthyindividuals AT wandb reliabilityofeyetrackingtoassessattentionalbiastothreateningwordsinhealthyindividuals AT traegera reliabilityofeyetrackingtoassessattentionalbiastothreateningwordsinhealthyindividuals AT gustins reliabilityofeyetrackingtoassessattentionalbiastothreateningwordsinhealthyindividuals AT mcauleyj reliabilityofeyetrackingtoassessattentionalbiastothreateningwordsinhealthyindividuals |