The reliability of eyetracking to assess attentional bias to threatening words in healthy individuals

Eyetracking is commonly used to investigate attentional bias. Although some studies have investigated the internal consistency of eyetracking, data are scarce on the test–retest reliability and agreement of eyetracking to investigate attentional bias. This study reports the test–retest reliability,...

Mô tả đầy đủ

Chi tiết về thư mục
Những tác giả chính: Skinner, I, Hübscher, M, Moseley, G, Lee, H, Wand, B, Traeger, A, Gustin, S, McAuley, J
Định dạng: Journal article
Ngôn ngữ:English
Được phát hành: Springer 2017
_version_ 1826294542532345856
author Skinner, I
Hübscher, M
Moseley, G
Lee, H
Wand, B
Traeger, A
Gustin, S
McAuley, J
author_facet Skinner, I
Hübscher, M
Moseley, G
Lee, H
Wand, B
Traeger, A
Gustin, S
McAuley, J
author_sort Skinner, I
collection OXFORD
description Eyetracking is commonly used to investigate attentional bias. Although some studies have investigated the internal consistency of eyetracking, data are scarce on the test–retest reliability and agreement of eyetracking to investigate attentional bias. This study reports the test–retest reliability, measurement error, and internal consistency of 12 commonly used outcome measures thought to reflect the different components of attentional bias: overall attention, early attention, and late attention. Healthy participants completed a preferential-looking eyetracking task that involved the presentation of threatening (sensory words, general threat words, and affective words) and nonthreatening words. We used intraclass correlation coefficients (ICCs) to measure test–retest reliability (ICC > .70 indicates adequate reliability). The ICCs(2, 1) ranged from –.31 to .71. Reliability varied according to the outcome measure and threat word category. Sensory words had a lower mean ICC (.08) than either affective words (.32) or general threat words (.29). A longer exposure time was associated with higher test–retest reliability. All of the outcome measures, except second-run dwell time, demonstrated low measurement error (≺6%). Most of the outcome measures reported high internal consistency (α ≻ .93). Recommendations are discussed for improving the reliability of eyetracking tasks in future research.
first_indexed 2024-03-07T03:47:16Z
format Journal article
id oxford-uuid:bfe8f5fb-d760-47b7-af7f-939fac66c9a9
institution University of Oxford
language English
last_indexed 2024-03-07T03:47:16Z
publishDate 2017
publisher Springer
record_format dspace
spelling oxford-uuid:bfe8f5fb-d760-47b7-af7f-939fac66c9a92022-03-27T05:51:01ZThe reliability of eyetracking to assess attentional bias to threatening words in healthy individualsJournal articlehttp://purl.org/coar/resource_type/c_dcae04bcuuid:bfe8f5fb-d760-47b7-af7f-939fac66c9a9EnglishSymplectic Elements at OxfordSpringer2017Skinner, IHübscher, MMoseley, GLee, HWand, BTraeger, AGustin, SMcAuley, JEyetracking is commonly used to investigate attentional bias. Although some studies have investigated the internal consistency of eyetracking, data are scarce on the test–retest reliability and agreement of eyetracking to investigate attentional bias. This study reports the test–retest reliability, measurement error, and internal consistency of 12 commonly used outcome measures thought to reflect the different components of attentional bias: overall attention, early attention, and late attention. Healthy participants completed a preferential-looking eyetracking task that involved the presentation of threatening (sensory words, general threat words, and affective words) and nonthreatening words. We used intraclass correlation coefficients (ICCs) to measure test–retest reliability (ICC > .70 indicates adequate reliability). The ICCs(2, 1) ranged from –.31 to .71. Reliability varied according to the outcome measure and threat word category. Sensory words had a lower mean ICC (.08) than either affective words (.32) or general threat words (.29). A longer exposure time was associated with higher test–retest reliability. All of the outcome measures, except second-run dwell time, demonstrated low measurement error (≺6%). Most of the outcome measures reported high internal consistency (α ≻ .93). Recommendations are discussed for improving the reliability of eyetracking tasks in future research.
spellingShingle Skinner, I
Hübscher, M
Moseley, G
Lee, H
Wand, B
Traeger, A
Gustin, S
McAuley, J
The reliability of eyetracking to assess attentional bias to threatening words in healthy individuals
title The reliability of eyetracking to assess attentional bias to threatening words in healthy individuals
title_full The reliability of eyetracking to assess attentional bias to threatening words in healthy individuals
title_fullStr The reliability of eyetracking to assess attentional bias to threatening words in healthy individuals
title_full_unstemmed The reliability of eyetracking to assess attentional bias to threatening words in healthy individuals
title_short The reliability of eyetracking to assess attentional bias to threatening words in healthy individuals
title_sort reliability of eyetracking to assess attentional bias to threatening words in healthy individuals
work_keys_str_mv AT skinneri thereliabilityofeyetrackingtoassessattentionalbiastothreateningwordsinhealthyindividuals
AT hubscherm thereliabilityofeyetrackingtoassessattentionalbiastothreateningwordsinhealthyindividuals
AT moseleyg thereliabilityofeyetrackingtoassessattentionalbiastothreateningwordsinhealthyindividuals
AT leeh thereliabilityofeyetrackingtoassessattentionalbiastothreateningwordsinhealthyindividuals
AT wandb thereliabilityofeyetrackingtoassessattentionalbiastothreateningwordsinhealthyindividuals
AT traegera thereliabilityofeyetrackingtoassessattentionalbiastothreateningwordsinhealthyindividuals
AT gustins thereliabilityofeyetrackingtoassessattentionalbiastothreateningwordsinhealthyindividuals
AT mcauleyj thereliabilityofeyetrackingtoassessattentionalbiastothreateningwordsinhealthyindividuals
AT skinneri reliabilityofeyetrackingtoassessattentionalbiastothreateningwordsinhealthyindividuals
AT hubscherm reliabilityofeyetrackingtoassessattentionalbiastothreateningwordsinhealthyindividuals
AT moseleyg reliabilityofeyetrackingtoassessattentionalbiastothreateningwordsinhealthyindividuals
AT leeh reliabilityofeyetrackingtoassessattentionalbiastothreateningwordsinhealthyindividuals
AT wandb reliabilityofeyetrackingtoassessattentionalbiastothreateningwordsinhealthyindividuals
AT traegera reliabilityofeyetrackingtoassessattentionalbiastothreateningwordsinhealthyindividuals
AT gustins reliabilityofeyetrackingtoassessattentionalbiastothreateningwordsinhealthyindividuals
AT mcauleyj reliabilityofeyetrackingtoassessattentionalbiastothreateningwordsinhealthyindividuals