Visual versus visual-inertial guidance in hawks pursuing terrestrial targets

The aerial interception behaviour of falcons is well modelled by a guidance law called proportional navigation, which commands steering at a rate proportional to the angular rate of the line-of-sight from predator to prey. Because the line-of-sight rate is defined in an inertial frame of reference, p...

Full description

Bibliographic Details
Main Authors: Kempton, J, Brighton, C, France, L, Klein Heerenbrink, M, Miñano, S, Shelton, J, Taylor, G
Format: Journal article
Language:English
Published: The Royal Society 2023
_version_ 1797110901213495296
author Kempton, J
Brighton, C
France, L
Klein Heerenbrink, M
Miñano, S
Shelton, J
Taylor, G
author_facet Kempton, J
Brighton, C
France, L
Klein Heerenbrink, M
Miñano, S
Shelton, J
Taylor, G
author_sort Kempton, J
collection OXFORD
description The aerial interception behaviour of falcons is well modelled by a guidance law called proportional navigation, which commands steering at a rate proportional to the angular rate of the line-of-sight from predator to prey. Because the line-of-sight rate is defined in an inertial frame of reference, proportional navigation must be implemented using visual-inertial sensor fusion. In contrast, the aerial pursuit behaviour of hawks chasing terrestrial targets is better modelled by a mixed guidance law combining information on the line-of-sight rate with information on the deviation angle between the attacker’s velocity and the line-of-sight. Here we ask whether this behaviour may be controlled using visual information alone. We use high-speed motion capture to record n=228 flights from N=4 Harris’ hawks Parabuteo unicinctus, and show that proportional navigation and mixed guidance both model their trajectories well. The mixed guidance law also models the data closely when visual-inertial information on the line-of-sight rate is replaced by visual information on the motion of the target relative to its background. Although the visual-inertial form of the mixed guidance law provides the closest fit, all three guidance laws provide an adequate phenomenological model of the behavioural data, whilst making different predictions on the physiological pathways involved.
first_indexed 2024-03-07T08:01:13Z
format Journal article
id oxford-uuid:e4ec2927-f3de-460a-a5f9-ceff14705e4b
institution University of Oxford
language English
last_indexed 2024-03-07T08:01:13Z
publishDate 2023
publisher The Royal Society
record_format dspace
spelling oxford-uuid:e4ec2927-f3de-460a-a5f9-ceff14705e4b2023-09-22T12:25:24ZVisual versus visual-inertial guidance in hawks pursuing terrestrial targetsJournal articlehttp://purl.org/coar/resource_type/c_dcae04bcuuid:e4ec2927-f3de-460a-a5f9-ceff14705e4bEnglishSymplectic ElementsThe Royal Society2023Kempton, JBrighton, CFrance, LKlein Heerenbrink, MMiñano, SShelton, JTaylor, GThe aerial interception behaviour of falcons is well modelled by a guidance law called proportional navigation, which commands steering at a rate proportional to the angular rate of the line-of-sight from predator to prey. Because the line-of-sight rate is defined in an inertial frame of reference, proportional navigation must be implemented using visual-inertial sensor fusion. In contrast, the aerial pursuit behaviour of hawks chasing terrestrial targets is better modelled by a mixed guidance law combining information on the line-of-sight rate with information on the deviation angle between the attacker’s velocity and the line-of-sight. Here we ask whether this behaviour may be controlled using visual information alone. We use high-speed motion capture to record n=228 flights from N=4 Harris’ hawks Parabuteo unicinctus, and show that proportional navigation and mixed guidance both model their trajectories well. The mixed guidance law also models the data closely when visual-inertial information on the line-of-sight rate is replaced by visual information on the motion of the target relative to its background. Although the visual-inertial form of the mixed guidance law provides the closest fit, all three guidance laws provide an adequate phenomenological model of the behavioural data, whilst making different predictions on the physiological pathways involved.
spellingShingle Kempton, J
Brighton, C
France, L
Klein Heerenbrink, M
Miñano, S
Shelton, J
Taylor, G
Visual versus visual-inertial guidance in hawks pursuing terrestrial targets
title Visual versus visual-inertial guidance in hawks pursuing terrestrial targets
title_full Visual versus visual-inertial guidance in hawks pursuing terrestrial targets
title_fullStr Visual versus visual-inertial guidance in hawks pursuing terrestrial targets
title_full_unstemmed Visual versus visual-inertial guidance in hawks pursuing terrestrial targets
title_short Visual versus visual-inertial guidance in hawks pursuing terrestrial targets
title_sort visual versus visual inertial guidance in hawks pursuing terrestrial targets
work_keys_str_mv AT kemptonj visualversusvisualinertialguidanceinhawkspursuingterrestrialtargets
AT brightonc visualversusvisualinertialguidanceinhawkspursuingterrestrialtargets
AT francel visualversusvisualinertialguidanceinhawkspursuingterrestrialtargets
AT kleinheerenbrinkm visualversusvisualinertialguidanceinhawkspursuingterrestrialtargets
AT minanos visualversusvisualinertialguidanceinhawkspursuingterrestrialtargets
AT sheltonj visualversusvisualinertialguidanceinhawkspursuingterrestrialtargets
AT taylorg visualversusvisualinertialguidanceinhawkspursuingterrestrialtargets