The use of intuitive and analytic reasoning styles by patients with persecutory delusions.

BACKGROUND AND OBJECTIVES: A previous study has shown an association of paranoid thinking with a reliance on rapid intuitive ('experiential') reasoning and less use of slower effortful analytic ('rational') reasoning. The objectives of the new study were to replicate the test of...

Полное описание

Библиографические подробности
Главные авторы: Freeman, D, Lister, R, Evans, N
Формат: Journal article
Язык:English
Опубликовано: Elsevier 2014
_version_ 1826280694820634624
author Freeman, D
Lister, R
Evans, N
author_facet Freeman, D
Lister, R
Evans, N
author_sort Freeman, D
collection OXFORD
description BACKGROUND AND OBJECTIVES: A previous study has shown an association of paranoid thinking with a reliance on rapid intuitive ('experiential') reasoning and less use of slower effortful analytic ('rational') reasoning. The objectives of the new study were to replicate the test of paranoia and reasoning styles in a large general population sample and to assess the use of these reasoning styles in patients with persecutory delusions. METHOD: 30 Patients with persecutory delusions in the context of a non-affective psychotic disorder and 1000 non-clinical individuals completed self-report assessments of paranoia and reasoning styles. RESULTS: The patients with delusions reported lower levels of both experiential and analytic reasoning than the non-clinical individuals (effect sizes small to moderate). Both self-rated ability and engagement with the reasoning styles were lower in the clinical group. Within the non-clinical group, greater levels of paranoia were associated with lower levels of analytic reasoning, but there was no association with experiential reasoning. LIMITATIONS: The study is cross-sectional and cannot determine whether the reasoning styles contribute to the occurrence of paranoia. It also cannot be determined whether the patient group's lower reasoning scores are specifically associated with the delusions. CONCLUSIONS: Clinical paranoia is associated with less reported use of analytic and experiential reasoning. This may reflect patients with current delusions being unconfident in their reasoning abilities or less aware of decision-making processes and hence less able to re-evaluate fearful cognitions. The dual process theory of reasoning may provide a helpful framework in which to discuss with patients decision-making styles.
first_indexed 2024-03-07T00:17:33Z
format Journal article
id oxford-uuid:7b5ea79f-d24c-47fb-9f9a-58caa2cfe0eb
institution University of Oxford
language English
last_indexed 2024-03-07T00:17:33Z
publishDate 2014
publisher Elsevier
record_format dspace
spelling oxford-uuid:7b5ea79f-d24c-47fb-9f9a-58caa2cfe0eb2022-03-26T20:50:11ZThe use of intuitive and analytic reasoning styles by patients with persecutory delusions.Journal articlehttp://purl.org/coar/resource_type/c_dcae04bcuuid:7b5ea79f-d24c-47fb-9f9a-58caa2cfe0ebEnglishSymplectic Elements at OxfordElsevier2014Freeman, DLister, REvans, NBACKGROUND AND OBJECTIVES: A previous study has shown an association of paranoid thinking with a reliance on rapid intuitive ('experiential') reasoning and less use of slower effortful analytic ('rational') reasoning. The objectives of the new study were to replicate the test of paranoia and reasoning styles in a large general population sample and to assess the use of these reasoning styles in patients with persecutory delusions. METHOD: 30 Patients with persecutory delusions in the context of a non-affective psychotic disorder and 1000 non-clinical individuals completed self-report assessments of paranoia and reasoning styles. RESULTS: The patients with delusions reported lower levels of both experiential and analytic reasoning than the non-clinical individuals (effect sizes small to moderate). Both self-rated ability and engagement with the reasoning styles were lower in the clinical group. Within the non-clinical group, greater levels of paranoia were associated with lower levels of analytic reasoning, but there was no association with experiential reasoning. LIMITATIONS: The study is cross-sectional and cannot determine whether the reasoning styles contribute to the occurrence of paranoia. It also cannot be determined whether the patient group's lower reasoning scores are specifically associated with the delusions. CONCLUSIONS: Clinical paranoia is associated with less reported use of analytic and experiential reasoning. This may reflect patients with current delusions being unconfident in their reasoning abilities or less aware of decision-making processes and hence less able to re-evaluate fearful cognitions. The dual process theory of reasoning may provide a helpful framework in which to discuss with patients decision-making styles.
spellingShingle Freeman, D
Lister, R
Evans, N
The use of intuitive and analytic reasoning styles by patients with persecutory delusions.
title The use of intuitive and analytic reasoning styles by patients with persecutory delusions.
title_full The use of intuitive and analytic reasoning styles by patients with persecutory delusions.
title_fullStr The use of intuitive and analytic reasoning styles by patients with persecutory delusions.
title_full_unstemmed The use of intuitive and analytic reasoning styles by patients with persecutory delusions.
title_short The use of intuitive and analytic reasoning styles by patients with persecutory delusions.
title_sort use of intuitive and analytic reasoning styles by patients with persecutory delusions
work_keys_str_mv AT freemand theuseofintuitiveandanalyticreasoningstylesbypatientswithpersecutorydelusions
AT listerr theuseofintuitiveandanalyticreasoningstylesbypatientswithpersecutorydelusions
AT evansn theuseofintuitiveandanalyticreasoningstylesbypatientswithpersecutorydelusions
AT freemand useofintuitiveandanalyticreasoningstylesbypatientswithpersecutorydelusions
AT listerr useofintuitiveandanalyticreasoningstylesbypatientswithpersecutorydelusions
AT evansn useofintuitiveandanalyticreasoningstylesbypatientswithpersecutorydelusions